Previous Page  15 / 56 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 15 / 56 Next Page
Page Background

Complex communication needs

www.speechpathologyaustralia.org.au

JCPSLP

Volume 14, Number 2 2012

69

Sally Clendon

(top) and

Marleen

Westerveld

This article

has been

peer-

reviewed

Keywords

AUGMENTATIVE

AND ALTERNATIVE

COMMUNICATION

LANGUAGE

DEVELOPMENT

VOCABULARY

WRITING

selected topics. In the first few years of school, students

are frequently asked to choose their own writing topics

(Richards & Sturm, 2010). Research examining the

vocabulary used when students write on self-selected

topics provides insight into what students do naturally

as beginning writers. One of these studies (Clendon &

Erickson, 2008) included NZ children.

The current project used two datasets to compare the

spoken and written vocabularies of typically developing 5-,

6-, and 7-year-old NZ children. This is the first project to

compare the vocabulary used across the modalities for the

purpose of informing vocabulary selection in AAC. The aim

was to identify similarities and differences between spoken

and written communication and to generate word lists

to assist AAC professionals as they engage in selecting,

prioritising, and organising vocabulary to support both face-

to-face and written communication in children with complex

communication needs.

Methodology

Written language database

The writing samples were obtained as part of the first

author’s dissertation research (see Clendon & Erickson,

2008). The participants included 106 children: 31 aged 5

years, 49 aged 6 years, and 26 aged 7 years. The children

were recruited from three NZ primary schools. They were in

the classrooms of teachers who provided regular (at least

three times per week) opportunities for children to write

about self-selected topics. All writing samples (

n

= 1225),

produced during self-selected writing sessions over a

6-week period, were photocopied for analysis. The

teachers were asked to provide translations of any

handwriting or spelling attempts that were unclear or

unconventional. The teachers were also asked to complete

context logs with descriptions of events that may have

influenced children’s writing topics and vocabulary.

Spoken language database

The spoken samples were collected as part of a NZ

database project (Westerveld, Gillon, & Miller, 2004). The

participants included 216 children: 63 aged 5 years, 95

aged 6 years, and 58 aged 7 years. All children were seen

at their kindergarten or school and engaged in a

conversation with an adult using the Westerveld and Gillon

Language Sampling Protocol (Westerveld & Gillon, 2002). In

this protocol, prompts are used to encourage the child to

talk about an object of his/her choice, family, and everyday

activities.

This project used two datasets to compare

the spoken and written vocabularies of

typically developing 5-, 6-, and 7-year-old

New Zealand children. Similarities and

differences between spoken and written

words and multiword sequences were

identified, and word lists were generated that

can be used to assist augmentative and

alternative communication (AAC)

professionals as they engage in selecting

vocabulary to support both face-to-face and

written communication of children with

complex communication needs.

V

ocabulary selection is one of the greatest challenges

facing professionals in the augmentative and

alternative communication (AAC) field (Clendon,

2006). Ensuring that children with complex communication

needs have access to the rich and varied vocabularies that

they need in order to develop mature language and literacy

skills is a difficult and often overwhelming process (Clendon

& Erickson, 2008).

Several authors have suggested that AAC professionals

employ a variety of strategies to guide their decision-making

around vocabulary selection (e.g., Beukelman, Jones, &

Rowan, 1989; Fallon, Light, & Paige, 2001). One strategy is

to refer to the word lists generated from research examining

the vocabularies of typically developing children. A number

of studies have examined the spoken vocabularies of

typically developing children for the purpose of informing

vocabulary selection in AAC (e.g., Ball, Marvin, Beukelman,

Lasker, & Rupp, 1999; Banajee, Dicarlo, & Stricklin, 2003;

Beukelman et al.,1989; Crestani, Clendon, & Hemsley,

2010; Fallon et al., 2001; Fried-Oken & More, 1992; Marvin,

Beukelman, & Bilyeu, 1994; Trembath, Balandin, & Togher,

2007). These studies have elicited samples across a variety

of linguistic contexts, such as playtime and mealtime, and

in response to oral narrative tasks. None of these studies

have included New Zealand (NZ) children.

In addition, three studies (Clendon & Erickson, 2008;

Clendon, Sturm, & Cali, 2012; McGinnis & Beukelman,

1989) have examined the written vocabularies of typically

developing children. In McGinnis and Beukelman (1989),

the writing samples were taken from letter writing

activities, science project assignments, and/or language

arts assignments. In Clendon and Erickson (2008) and

Clendon et al. (2012), the writing samples were on self-

Research note

Comparing spoken and written vocabulary use in typically

developing children: Informing vocabulary selection for

children with complex communication needs

Sally Clendon and Marleen Westerveld