Previous Page  32 / 218 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 32 / 218 Next Page
Page Background

30

Chapter 2

no effect in the group of subjects with already high dopamine levels. Furthermore, a number

of studies have shown that the effects of a dopamine D2 receptor agonist can be explained by

natural variation in the gene coding for the dopamine D2 receptor (Kirsch et al., 2006; Cohen

et al., 2007). Subjects carrying the

Taq

1A1 variant of the allele (A1+ subjects) have ~30%

fewer dopamine D2 receptors in the striatum and exhibit impairments in reward processing,

compared with those not carrying this allele (A1- subjects). Cohen and colleagues (2007)

assessed whether this genetic predisposition could explain individual responses to a dopamine

receptor agonist during reward processing. To this end they used a reversal learning paradigm,

which requires flexible adaptation of behaviour when a previously rewarded stimulus is no

longer rewarded and a newrule needs tobe learned. In the placebo condition, the lowdopamine

receptor group (A1+ subjects) performed worse than the A1- group. However, administration

of the dopamine D2 receptor agonist cabergoline improved rule-learning performance in the

subset of subjects with genetically determined low dopamine receptor density (A1+), but

the dopamine D2 receptor agonist impaired performance in those already performing well

under placebo (the A1- group). This effect was accompanied by opposite effects in reward-

related neural responses in regions of the reward network (the medial orbitofrontal cortex

and striatum): Administration of the D2 receptor agonist increased activity in these regions in

subjects with low reward-related activity under placebo (A1+), while it had the opposite effect

in those with already high baseline reward-related activity (A1-).

As was discussed in

chapter 1

, in addition to predicting individual differences indrug response,

natural (genetic) variation between individuals can also explain individual differences in task

performance (e.g. (Frank et al., 2007; Dreher et al., 2009; Aarts et al., 2010; Colzato et al.,

2010a; Stelzel et al., 2010) (

box 2.2c

). In

chapters 3 and 4

, I exploited individual variability

in the gene coding for the dopamine transporter (DAT) to account for inter-individual

variability in task performance, neural signalling, and drug response. Task-related differences

in performance or neural signalling as a function of variation in this genotype can be taken to

suggest that dopamine is involved in the studied process.

Evidence for a role for dopamine in the integration of reward motivation and cognitive control

has been provided by a number of studies, and much of this evidence is reviewed in

chapter

1

of this thesis. Previous work that also employed the rewarded task-switching paradigm

presented in this thesis (

box 2.3

) has shown that reward can modulate flexible control in

the context of task switching (Aarts et al., 2010). The anticipation of a reward (i.e. high vs.

low reward cue) increased neural responses in the ventral striatum, while the integration

between reward and task switching was associated with increased signalling in the caudate

nucleus. Interestingly, these signals correlated, suggesting that communication between the

ventral and dorsal striatum may mediate the information transfer from reward regions to

cognitive control regions (

figures 1.2c, d and 2.1, box 2.1

). Dopamine-dependent effects in

this latter study were revealed by showing that inter-individual differences in signalling in

the caudate nucleus depended crucially on individual differences in dopamine signalling,

measured by exploiting differences in the

DAT1

genotype. Although these results provide