Previous Page  24 / 60 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 24 / 60 Next Page
Page Background

22

ACQ

Volume 13, Number 1 2011

ACQ

uiring knowledge in speech, language and hearing

encouraged to identify the target phoneme in the context of

one of the target words, to identify and label the sounds and

names for the target phoneme, as well as to practise

accurate production of the target phoneme in words. For the

PA activities that incorporated word-initial /s/ cluster speech

targets, participants were requested only to identify the /s/.

The developmental appropriateness of this task is supported

by the work of Stahl and Murray (1994) who report that

kindergarteners and first graders were able to identify the

initial phoneme in CCVC words with a mean accuracy of

72%.

Morphosyntax and speech intervention

The MS intervention program alternated between two

sessions targeting a morphological structure followed by two

sessions of speech intervention the week after. The three

morphological structures that were targeted during

intervention were: the regular third person singular form of

verbs (e.g.,

blows

,

makes

,

eats

); the regular past tense of

verbs (e.g.,

looked

,

washed

,

talked

); and the copula form of

the verb ‘to be’ (e.g.,

I am tired

,

They are mine

,

It is empty

).

Each morphological structure was targeted for four

intervention sessions in total, two during each cycle of

therapy. Within the session, participants were encouraged to

attempt a range of words that used the target morpheme,

but no specific vocabulary list was prescribed for these

sessions. The purpose of the sessions that focused on

morphological structure was not to teach specific clusters

but to create a greater awareness of word endings and the

copula through exposure to a variety of words that included

the target morpheme. The morphosyntax sessions began

with auditory bombardment of the target morphological

structure in the context of a picture book. Focused

stimulation and elicited production activities were then

conducted in the context of group play activities. Feedback

included explicit modeling of the correct production of words

containing the target morpheme as is typical in language-

based approaches.

During the speech sessions, participants received

individualised targets: final consonant deletion for Matt and

velar fronting for Ben. No consonant clusters were targeted

during these sessions. The speech sessions followed a

similar format to the morphosyntax sessions with auditory

bombardment of the target sounds for the session in the

context of a picture book. This was followed by activities

that maximised opportunities for production practice of the

speech targets.

Main measures

Measure of cluster development

The 26-word cluster probe (see Appendix) was administered

at three different points in time: pre-intervention, immediately

post-intervention, and 3 months post-intervention. The

cluster probe that was administered immediately post-

intervention took place approximately 4½ months after

administration of the pre-intervention cluster probe.

Measure of additional target phonological

error pattern

Each participant was administered one of two 16-word

production probes at two different points in time: pre-

intervention, and immediately post-intervention. One

production probe measured accuracy of final consonants

and the other production probe measured accuracy of velar

stops in both word-initial position (n = 11) and word-final

position (n = 5). Pre-intervention accuracy on these

– velar fronting, gliding of /r/. From these phonological error

patterns, one additional intervention target was selected for

each child: final consonant deletion for Aaron and Matt, and

velar fronting for Mike and Ben.

General procedure

Participants attended a total of 24 therapy sessions (of at

least 45 minutes), two sessions a week, in groups of four

children in a university clinic setting. Four children who

participated in a larger study that investigated the efficacy of

two types of treatment for children with speech and

language disorder (Gillon & Tyler, 2007) were also involved in

the therapy sessions but were not included in this study as

they did not have difficulty producing consonant clusters.

Therapy was provided in two 6-week blocks, with a 6-week

break between treatment blocks. In the period between the

immediate post-intervention testing and the testing 3 months

post-intervention, participants did not receive any direct

intervention for the production of speech sounds but

continued with their regular early education program at

kindergarten or play centre. Intervention was implemented

by the third author and speech pathology students under the

supervision of a certified speech pathologist.

Intervention

Participants were randomly assigned to take part in one of

the two types of intervention: (1) integrated phonological

awareness and speech (PAS) intervention, or (2) a

morphosyntax and speech (MS) intervention. The PAS

intervention program (Gillon & McNeill, 2007) targeted

speech production, phonological awareness, and letter

knowledge, but not expressive language. The MS

intervention program (Haskill, Tyler, & Tolbert, 2001) targeted

various morphosyntactic structures and speech production,

but not phonological awareness. Both programs provided

therapy for the same frequency, length, and number of

sessions. A similar number of target words were introduced

for each speech target for both types of intervention. At least

10 attempts of the target phonological or morphological

structure were elicited from each child during each

intervention session. Parents, siblings, and/or caregivers

observed both types of intervention from observation rooms

behind one-way mirrors. Although there was no specific

home practice provided for either intervention program,

parents were not discouraged from practising target sounds

or activities at home. It should be noted, however, that the

children in the MS group received only 8 sessions that

included coda clusters (the target morpheme for four

sessions was the copula) whereas the children in the PAS

group received 12 sessions that targeted onset clusters.

Phonological awareness and speech intervention

The aim of the PAS intervention program was to facilitate

letter knowledge, and early phoneme awareness

development, and to decrease target speech error patterns.

(See

www.education.canterbury.ac.nz/people/gillon/

integrated_phonological_awareness.shtml for a detailed

discussion of the rationale and implementation of this

approach.) The phonological error pattern of reducing word

initial /s/ clusters was targeted for the first 6 weeks of

intervention. During the second 6-week block of therapy,

participants received individualised targets: final consonant

deletion for Aaron and velar fronting for Mike. Phonological

awareness instruction was incorporated into the speech

production activities in many of the session activities. For

example, within the same activity, participants were