Previous Page  23 / 60 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 23 / 60 Next Page
Page Background www.speechpathologyaustralia.org.au

ACQ

Volume 11, Number 3 2009

149

Mental health

Andrew

Whitehouse

Keywords

AUTISM

SPECIFIC

LANGUAGE

IMPAIRMENT

PRAGMATIC

LANGUAGE

IMPAIRMENT

DIAGNOSIS

ADULT

OUTCOME

underlying aetiology of each condition) and clinical practice

(identifying interventions that may be more effective with

different groups of individuals).

Differential diagnosis

The current diagnostic approach is to differentiate “specific

language impairments” (SLI) from “non-specific language

impairments”. The former category includes those children

whose difficulties are restricted to the language domain,

whereas the latter category includes difficulties that are

associated with a broader condition, such as autism.

“Textbook” cases of SLI and autism are relatively easy to

identify and differentiate. The broad communicative

difficulties of individuals with autism (affecting both structural

and pragmatic aspects of communication) contrast with SLI,

in which there is a relatively specific deficit in the

development of linguistic skills. However, research over the

past decade has raised concerns over the validity of this

diagnostic divide. For example, many children have

behavioural characteristics that could be considered

“intermediate” between SLI and autism. Autism, in particular,

is widely recognised as a broad spectrum of disorders,

ranging from autism at the severe end of the continuum, to

pervasive developmental disorder – not otherwise specified

(PDD-NOS; behaviours characteristic of autism, but not at a

quantitative and/or qualitative severity to reach full criteria for

autism) and Asperger syndrome (behaviours characteristic of

autism, without any clinically significant delay in language or

cognitive development) at the less severe end. The

spectrum-nature of this condition has led to the now widely

used term

autism spectrum conditions

(ASC), which refers to

individuals with either a diagnosis of autism or PDD-NOS. It

is also not uncommon to observe a child with pragmatic

difficulties, but without the repetitive behaviours and marked

social deficits that are required to meet criteria for an ASC.

Such children have led to the additional diagnostic category

of pragmatic language impairment (PLI). Originally referred to

as semantic-pragmatic disorder, there has been a transition

to the alternative label of PLI, particularly in the United

Kingdom, due to findings that semantic and pragmatic

deficits do not always occur in combination (Bishop, 1998).

Although the current paper will adopt ASC and PLI to refer to

the syndromes described above, it is important to note that

the terms are not currently recognised in international

diagnostic guidelines.

The jury is still out on the ecological validity of the PLI

diagnostic category. Perhaps the most comprehensive

Specific language impairment and autism are

considered distinct developmental disorders.

However, while “textbook” cases of these

conditions can be differentiated with little

difficulty, there is a substantial proportion of

children who display “intermediate”

characteristics. One such example is the

group of children who exhibit pragmatic

language difficulties in the absence of other

autism-like behaviours, so called pragmatic

language impairment. There is contention as

to whether the difficulties of these children are

best considered a mild form of autism, or

whether they represent a diagnostic category

in their own right (so-called pragmatic

language impairment). This paper highlights

current thinking in the diagnostic

differentiation of these disorders, using

evidence from a longitudinal study

investigating the language, psychosocial, and

mental health outcomes in adulthood of

children with each condition. The findings

reinforce the validity of the pragmatic

language impairment diagnosis, and suggest

that adult psychosocial outcomes can be

predicted from their childhood language

profile. Mental ill-health was one outcome that

was relatively common among the adults with

a history of a communication disorder,

suggesting that a good working knowledge of

psychiatric conditions is beneficial for speech

pathology practice.

C

ommunication disorders are, by their very nature,

heterogeneous; it is rare to find two individuals with

identical difficulties. Part of the issue here is that

communication is a broad category, encompassing both

structural (e.g., phonology, morphology, and semantics) and

pragmatic aspects of language (i.e., how language is used

in context). One of the goals of research in this area has

been to provide “order”, by identifying diagnostic categories

into which children can be grouped. Differential diagnosis

has benefits for both research (e.g., helping to elucidate the

Differentiating between

childhood communication

disorders

Implications for language and psychosocial outcomes

Andrew Whitehouse