Previous Page  6 / 60 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 6 / 60 Next Page
Page Background

4

ACQ

Volume 12, Number 1 2010

ACQ

uiring knowledge in speech, language and hearing

monkeys, firing of mirror neurones has been shown to

increase learning and success in new motor activities without

the monkey actually practising the task concerned (Rizzolatti

& Craighero, 2004). The existence of these neurones would

partially explain why modelling is an effective and natural part

of many motor interventions and why imagining yourself

completing an action can improve performance on this

action, a concept well known in elite sports. Indeed, work in

people with stroke has suggested that improved motor

performance can be achieved through detailed imagining of

movement (e.g., Yoo, Park, & Chung, 2001). In addition, one

PML suggests that watching someone else learn how to do

a task is more efficient than an expert modelling the

behaviour (Hebert & Landin, 1994) and this principle may be

explained by mirror neurones. It is possible that watching

someone else learn leads the mirror neurones to simulate

learning the action. Thus in the future we may develop a

theoretical rather than economic justification for group

intervention.

Integral stimulation (Strand & Skinder, 1999) is an example

of a treatment which uses these hypothesised mirror

neurones to aid learning. Two principles of this approach that

may utilise mirror neurones include: 1) the clinician should

sit very close to and directly opposite the patient so that the

clinician’s face occupies most of the patient’s visual field, and

2) in the early stages of treatment, the clinician and patient

say the target sounds simultaneously.

Conclusions

There are other areas of emerging knowledge for which we

do not have space in this paper, but which are equally

fascinating. These include constraint induced change,

transcranial magnetic stimulation (as described in Goozée,

this issue), and the role of the undamaged hemisphere in

inhibiting recovery from brain damage. It is exciting to see

areas of speech pathology practice that have been relatively

dormant changing through the newly available understanding

of how skilled movement is learnt and how the brain

functions. Watch this space.

References

Ballard, K.J., Robin, D.A., McCabe, P.J., & McDonald, J.

(2009).

Treating dysprosody in childhood apraxia of speech

.

Paper presented at Speech Pathology Australia Annual

National Conference, May 2009, Adelaide.

Doidge, N. (2007).

The brain that changes itself

.

Melbourne: Scribe.

Hebert, E. P., & Landin, D. (1994). Effects of a learning

model and augmented feedback on tennis skill acquisition.

Research Quarterly for Exercise & Sport

,

65

, 250–257.

Hodges, N. J., & Lee, T. D. (1999). The role of augmented

information prior to learning a bimanual visual-motor

coordination task: Do instructions of the movement pattern

facilitate learning relative to discovery learning?

British

Journal of Psychology

,

90

, 389–403.

Iacobini, M. (2005). Neural mechanisms of imitation.

Current Opinion in Neurobiology

,

15

(6), 632–637.

Maas, E., Robin, D. A., Austermann Hula, S. N.,

Freedman, S. E., Wulf, G., & Ballard, K. J. (2008). Principles

of motor learning in treatment of motor speech disorders.

American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology

,

17

(3),

277–298.

carrier structure. In this way we can use recent theoretical

research to guide practice in the absence of higher level

evidence.

Neural plasticity

The two-cutting edge research areas presented here, neural

plasticity and mirror neurones, underpin PML and provide

new ways of thinking about motor based intervention across

the board. The concept of neural plasticity is one which has

emerged in neurology in the past few years and is the focus

of a recent popular science book

The Brain That Changes

Itself

(Doidge, 2007).

As late as the early 1990s it was widely believed that the

brain did not repair itself after stroke or head injury, but we

now know that brains have both adaptive and maladaptive

repair processes operating continuously which can be

harnessed in the rehabilitation process. Neural plasticity

refers to these constantly engaged adaptive processes

which allow us to learn new skills as a result of sensory

input. This sensory information comes from our five primary

senses but also, and importantly for motor learning, from

our proprioception system including stretch receptors in

muscles. When we damage our brains, or the sensory inputs

to them, these adaptive processes continue to function

and react to the distorted sensory input produced by the

damage. This means that in the absence of normal function

(motor or otherwise), the brain starts to use the available, but

incorrect, information as the input and thus to lay down new

learning based on this distorted input. The result is shifting

of allocation of neurological resources, learning of new and

disabling motor patterns, and ongoing loss of function. The

longer this disrupted learning continues, the “better” these

maladapted motor patterns are learnt (Pascual-Leone,

Amedi, Fregni, & Merabet, 2005).

It now seems clear that rehabilitation should start on

renewal of competent function as soon as possible and

certainly within days of the initial neurological insult as the

brain starts to change within 3–4 days of the changed

input. To delay is to allow maladaptive learning to take place

through reduced sensory input and through new motor

patterns which may be created by compensatory strategies.

In physiotherapy this means that comatose patients may

have their limbs moved and muscles stretched and this not

only helps prevent deep vein thrombosis (a medical goal)

but also provides the brain with sensory input. This input is

thought to help maintain brain function for the inert limbs and

to prevent maladaptive neural plasticity from using the part

of the sensory motor cortex allocated for the limb concerned

for another function.

So how will neural plasticity change speech pathology

practice? We might hypothesise that research will show

that in treatment of dysarthria, the sooner you start near

normal behaviours the better, or that with adults we need

to use errorless learning so that maladaptive neurological

changes are suppressed. Neural plasticity is emerging as a

strong argument for both early and continued high frequency

intervention in all aspects of motor learning.

Mirror neurones

Researchers are interested in a neurological construct known

as “mirror neurones”. Mirror neurones fire when we watch

someone else do an action and when we hear a sound

commonly associated with an action (Iacobini, 2005). In