Previous Page  21 / 68 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 21 / 68 Next Page
Page Background www.speechpathologyaustralia.org.au

ACQ

Volume 11, Number 2 2009

83

corresponding passage. The results indicated that 86% of

the questions used in the GORT–4 were answered correctly

more than 25% of the time (i.e., above chance level). No

significant correlations were found between performance on

the PI items and other spoken language comprehension test

measures, and performance on the PI items was not

sensitive to reading disability (as identified by word

recognition difficulties). Keenan and Betjemann (2006)

concluded that performance on the reading comprehension

measure of the GORT–4 yields information about the child’s

previous general world knowledge, rather than the child’s

ability to derive meaning from a written text.

developmental stage the child is at might influence the child’s

test performance. Four different reading comprehension

tests were administered to a sample of 510 children, ranging

in age from 8 to 18 years. The tests were:

GORT–3 (see table 1);

Qualitative Reading Inventory

– 3 (Leslie & Caldwell,

2001), in which the students read long passages out

loud; it involves two assessments of comprehension:

open-ended short-answer questions and retelling of one

of the passages;

Peabody Individual Achievement Test

(Dunn & Markwardt,

1970, cited in Keenan et al., 2008); the student silently

reads single sentences and short passages and indicates

comprehension by selecting from four pictures the one

that best fits the meaning of the sentence/passage just

read;

WRMT–R Passage Comprehension subtest (see table 1).

In addition, children’s listening comprehension (at passage

level) and word recognition skills were assessed. Consistent

with previous studies, it was found that tests using a cloze

format were more heavily dependent on word recognition

skill. More particularly, tests using very short passages

(one to two sentences) were the most sensitive to word

recognition ability. Keenan et al. (2008) reasoned that the

longer the passages are, the more chance the child has

to use the context (and thus demonstrate his listening

comprehension skills) to understand the passage, even if he

has trouble decoding one or two specific words. In contrast,

in very short passages, a child may fail to understand

the meaning of the material when unable to decode one

or two keywords. Unfortunately, these researchers did

not investigate whether oral or silent reading of the text

influenced task performance. It can be argued that reading

out loud may distract some children from attending to the

meaning of the text. Further research is required to test this

hypothesis.

When examining developmental differences (as a function

of chronological age and of reading age) in test performance,

Keenan et al. (2008) found that, consistent with the simple

view of reading, word recognition skills accounted for more

variance in reading comprehension in the younger age group

(mean age 9;1 years) than in the older age group (mean age

13;1). In addition, the findings demonstrated that tests using

cloze formats were more dependent on word recognition

ability in young children or in children with reading difficulties

than those tests using longer passages and question and

answer formats.

Passage dependency and previous

world knowledge

Question-and-answer tests can be divided into multiple

choice tasks and open-ended-question tasks. For example,

the GORT–4 uses multiple choice questions following the

reading of a passage, whereas the NARA requires the child

to answer a set of open-ended questions about a passage,

immediately after the child has read the passage out loud.

Keenan and Betjemann (2006) conducted a study to

investigate if the multiple choice format that is used by the

GORT–4 would be passage dependent. Passage dependent

(PD) questions can only be answered correctly by using

information from the passage, whereas passage

independent (PI) questions can also be answered without

reading the text. A total of 77 undergraduate students in

Education participated in a passageless administration of the

GORT–4. This procedure involved asking the students the

comprehension questions without exposing them to the

The contribution of pre-existing content-relevant

knowledge was highlighted in a study investigating the

comprehension and memory for spoken and written texts

of 181 adults, aged between 18 and 86 years (Hambrick

& Engle, 2002). Hambrick and Engle found that the

participants’ knowledge of the game of baseball significantly

facilitated their performance on questions (tapping their

memory of the game details) following exposure to a tape-

recorded narration of a baseball match. Participants who

were familiar with the game of baseball were better able to

answer questions related to the progress of the game as well

as more specific questions related to the performance of the

batsmen.

Test taking strategies

To determine if children’s test taking strategies would

influence their performance on a reading comprehension

test, Bornholt (2002) administered the

Test of Reading

Comprehension

(TORCH; Mossenson, Hill, & Masters, 1987)

to 30 children (aged 9 and 10) attending year 4 of their local

primary school. The TORCH uses a story booklet and an

answer booklet and children have to fill in blanks left in a

passage. In addition, children’s test-taking strategies were

observed, including procedural initiative, task involvement

and item responses (e.g., number of questions attempted).

Following the pre-test, all children participated in two 1-hour

class discussions about test-taking strategies and were

reassessed in week 4. Results indicated that most

improvement in reading test scores was observed for

children with initially poor test-taking strategies. Post-

intervention these children showed higher test completion

rates, and were more likely to attempt various items rather

than persisting with the items in a set order. Increased

awareness of this possibility in clinical practice may thus help

improve the accuracy of diagnosis.

Summary

This paper has discussed issues that may influence

children’s performance on tests of reading comprehension.

Research clearly indicates that the choice of reading

comprehension assessment in clinical and research practice

is critical as some tests may incorrectly identify children as

demonstrating good or poor reading comprehension skills

(Cutting & Scarborough, 2006; Keenan et al., 2008). The test

format plays an important role. To ensure adequate detection

of a child’s strengths and weaknesses in word recognition

Box 1. The simple view of reading

Reading comprehension = Word recognition X Listening

comprehension

Source: Gough & Tunmer, 1986