Previous Page  23 / 68 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 23 / 68 Next Page
Page Background

Literacy

www.speechpathologyaustralia.org.au

ACQ

Volume 11, Number 2 2009

85

Julie Marinac

This article

has been

peer-

reviewed

Keywords

diagnosis

dyslexia

evidence

based

practice

phonological

awareness

reading

impairment

secondary

school

appear that this inexplicable and unexpected failure to

achieve age-appropriate literacy skills has not changed over

time in nature, occurrence or presentation. What appears to

have changed is the social acceptability of the terminology

used to describe literacy learning failures, and our

understanding of this phenomenon.

There is general consensus that dyslexia is a complex

syndrome that moves beyond the traditional “reading” to

“reading-related processes” (i.e., beyond “reading” to include

spelling, and written language), and changes in presentation

with age and learning experiences (e.g., Benson, 1994;

Blachman, 1997; Grigorenko, Wood, Meyer, & Pauls, 2000;

Oakhill & Beard, 1999; Richardson & Weydell, 2003). A

further complication to the terminology and definitional

debate has been introduced in the 2000 revision of the

Diagnostic and Statistical Handbook of Mental Disorders

(DSM–IV American Psychiatric Association). This reference

text, which provides definitive clinical diagnostic guidance

for medical professionals, no longer includes “dyslexia” per

se but rather describes two learning disorders that correlate

with dyslexia – Reading Disorder (RD, 315.00) and Disorder

of Written Expression (DWE, 315.2) (DSM–IV TR, 2000).

Under this system, students who experience difficulty in both

reading and spelling could be diagnosed as having either

co-morbid or sequential disorders. For example, those who

have difficulty acquiring age appropriate reading skills before

demonstrating delays in written language and/or spelling

would be diagnosed as having RD. If the student then

demonstrates functional reading abilities but still experiences

difficulties in producing age appropriate written work, the

diagnosis would become DWE (e.g., sequential disorders).

Diagnostic subtypes of dyslexia

Notwithstanding these terminological issues, those who

work in the field rely on diagnostic subtypes of dyslexia to

guide assessment and intervention (Morris et al., 1998). At

least three primary subtypes have been reported: 1) “visual”

or “visual-perception” (Warnke, 1999), 2) “surface” dyslexia

(Castles & Coltheart, 1993), and 3) “deep” or “phonological”

dyslexia (Stanovich, Siegel, & Gottardo, 1997).

Visual dyslexia

The first category involves the letter reversals that suggest

the classic dyslexia of “strephosymbolia” (Orton, 1995).

Anecdotal evidence regarding the success of therapy (e.g.,

the introduction of “coloured glasses”, etc.) has been

reported, but Evans (1998) suggests that such visual

confusion, in isolation, should not even be defined as

Dyslexia, as a reading disability, has been

widely studied with consistent evidence being

reported regarding underlying deficits,

aetiology, and effects. Almost all of this

research has focused on the primary school

sector where students are still developing

literacy skills. Very little research is available

to guide those who work with students who

have similar deficits in secondary school. This

can lead to a situation in which data taken

from the developmental population are applied

to students who are expected to have mature

rather than emerging literacy skills. This report

provides an overview of different types of

dyslexia and summarises the research relating

to dyslexia in secondary school students. It

proposes that secondary school and adult

populations present with phonological

processing (PP) based dyslexia rather than

developmental reading impairments, and

presents clinical implications for speech

pathologists and teachers.

Defining dyslexia

Dyslexia is defined as a “complex, biologically rooted

behavioural condition resulting from an impairment of

reading-related processes and manifested in difficulties

related to the mastery of reading up to the level of population

norms under the conditions of adequate education and a

normal developmental environment” (Grigorenko 2001, p.

94). It is a complex disorder, found in children, adolescents

and adults, demonstrated by unexpected difficulty in learning

to read that is not attributable to general cognitive delay,

psychiatric or neurologic disorders, sensory impairment or

inadequate instruction (World Federation of Neurology,

1968). Current practices may lead to the use of alternative

terminology or a complete avoidance of such “labelling”. For

example, reading difficulties or impairment may be used, as

the term dyslexia “has often been avoided in educational

practice because of its predominant emphasis on within-

child causative factors and its perceived effects on social

policy” (Reason, Frederickson, Heffernan, Martin, & Woods,

1999, p. 16). From the evidence in the literature, it would

Dyslexia in secondary

school students

Evidence from the literature

Julie Marinac