Background Image
Table of Contents Table of Contents
Previous Page  35 / 64 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 35 / 64 Next Page
Page Background www.speechpathologyaustralia.org.au

JCPSLP

Volume 17, Number 3 2015

145

contingent communicative style was a positive one.

In contrast to a contingent communicative environment

is one with increased parental control. A longitudinal study

investigating parental control conducted by Lederberg and

Everhart (2000) compared the number of directives given

to 20 children who were D/HH and 20 children who were

NH, at 22 months and 3 years of age. The study involved

three measures of maternal directives. The number of direct

behavioural commands (“Stop that.”), was significantly

different between the groups. At both points in time, the

mothers of children with a hearing loss were significantly

more likely to use the direct behavioural commands, with

the number used by the mothers of children who were

D/HH the same as the number the mothers of the NH

children used at 22 months (Lederberg & Everhart, 2000).

The authors attribute the increased maternal control to

the delayed language abilities of the children who were

D/HH. Lam and Kitamura (2010) reported similar results

in a twin study, linking differences in maternal control to

the decreased responsiveness of the twin who was D/

HH. To the twin with a hearing loss, the mother was less

responsive, more controlling, and more directive during

interactions. Despite the ability of a twin study to more

closely control for individual variation and environmental

factors, the generalisability of the study is limited, and the

findings were not supported by statistical analyses. These

studies indicate the likelihood of a difference in caregiver

behaviour and control due to the impact of hearing loss on

child behaviour.

An important measure of the quality of HLE is the

presence of rich conversational experiences, including

opportunities to learn linguistic rules (Chouinard &

Clark, 2003). Supportive caregiver behaviours such as

expansions, repeats, recasts, and requests for clarification

enable children to learn language during conversation

(Chouinard & Clark, 2003). Ruter (2011) found that for

21 children following cochlear implantations, parental

expansions increased their acquisition of grammatical

structures, with a significant correlation between an

expansion of a grammatical structure and the children’s

subsequent usage. Children who are D/HH have more

profound difficulties in syntax and grammar compared

with vocabulary acquisition (Boons et al., 2013) and

therefore this study highlights the valuable role that parental

expansions have for language development and informing

intervention strategies in this population.

The results of these studies can be grouped into three

major themes: those indicating differences in parental

conversational control (Lam & Kitamura, 2010; Lederberg &

Everhart, 2000), parent–child responsiveness (Harrigan &

Nikolopoulos, 2002; Morgan et al., 2014; Most et al., 2010),

and the richness of the linguistic environment (Ruter, 2011).

Interaction quantity

The number of adult words a child is exposed to in the HLE

is acknowledged as a key facilitator of language acquisition

(Zimmerman et al., 2009). In terms of typical language

development, the number of adult words a child hears has

been found to be relatively stable, while conversational

turns and child vocalisations increase as the child ages and

language skills develop (Greenwood, Thiemann-Bourque,

Walker, Buzhardt & Gilkerson, 2010). Extrapolating these

findings to children who are D/HH, a reduction in

conversational turns and child vocalisations compared to

children who are NH may be expected, due to decreased

spoken language competence in this population.

Additionally, no significant differences in the number of adult

words may be anticipated. However, according to

experiences (Vigil et al., 2005). The scope of the current

study is therefore closely underpinned by the theory of

emergentism. This theory acknowledges the interaction

between the “external language environment and the

internal learning capabilities of the child” (Morgan et

al., 2014, p. 47). Emergentism indicates that there is a

likely compounding effect of a less supportive linguistic

environment in addition to language learning difficulties

associated with hearing loss. Consequently, the quality and

quantity of interaction in the HLE is considered in this paper.

Interaction quality

Demonstrating emergentism theory in this population, Lam

and Kitamura (2010) summarised research surrounding the

impact of hearing loss on the communicative behaviour of

caregivers of children who are D/HH. They suggested

caregivers are more controlling and dominant during

conversation, and that communicative breakdowns occur

more frequently due to decreased responsiveness from the

child (Lam & Kitamura, 2010). Similarly, Morgan et al. (2014)

examined the quality of conversational turns between 30

children who were D/HH and 19 children who were NH

aged between 17 and 35 months, and their caregivers. The

measures of conversational quality were based on those

proposed by Ensor and Hughes (2008), and included

coding exchanges as connected, initiated, failed, or unclear.

Connectedness is a measure of how semantically related

an utterance is to the previous turn. It provides an indication

of how often topics are continued, and how responsive

conversational partners are to each other’s communicative

attempts (Ensor & Hughes, 2008). As such, it can be used

to measure how supportive the HLE is for language

development, as children engage more enthusiastically and

frequently in topics initiated by them (Harrigan &

Nikolopoulos, 2002). In contrast, more failed or unclear

utterances indicate less successful interaction experiences

and conversational partners who are less responsive

(Harrigan & Nikolopoulos, 2002). The study found the NH

group displayed more connected conversational turns,

initiated communication more often, and had fewer failed or

unclear turns (Morgan et al., 2014). Although the study did

not account for non-verbal communication, the authors

suggest the conversational exchanges were “impoverished”

(Morgan et al., 2014, p 47).

These findings are supported by Most, Shina-August,

and Meilijson (2010). Out of 24 children receiving

amplification, appropriate connected interactions were not

displayed consistently between any children who were D/

HH and their caregivers. In contrast, such interactions were

displayed by 11 of the 13 NH children and their caregivers.

It should be noted that the authors viewed this discrepancy

as a difference in pragmatic function between the two

groups, and they acknowledged the impact on the success

of the communicative interactions.

According to the literature, the quality of interaction in

the HLE is improved when caregivers expand on their

child’s utterances, use self-talk, promote conversational

turn-taking, limit parental initiations, and follow their child’s

communicative initiations (Vigil et al., 2005). A study

conducted by Harrigan and Nikolopoulos (2002) involved

teaching caregivers of children with cochlear implants these

supportive behaviours. Parental utterances were assigned

to one of two categories: an initiation or a response. Post-

course, the parents responded significantly more to their

child rather than initiating a conversational turn (Harrigan

& Nikolopoulos, 2002). Although the study did not assess

the impact this might have had on the children’s language

abilities, the authors suggested that the shift towards a