Background Image
Table of Contents Table of Contents
Previous Page  37 / 64 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 37 / 64 Next Page
Page Background www.speechpathologyaustralia.org.au

JCPSLP

Volume 17, Number 3 2015

147

The audio file was transcribed into a Word document

and analysed using the following codes: a successful

child initiation, successful caregiver initiation, connected

utterance, failed utterance, behavioural directive, or

caregiver expansion. The ratio of successful child to

caregiver initiations was also calculated. These measures

were selected as their presence, or absence, provides

an indication of how supportive the HLE is of language

acquisition. See the Appendix for the definitions adhered to

for these codes. An inter-rater reliability analysis using the

Kappa statistic was performed. A total of 297 utterances

from six participants’ recordings were independently

coded for comparison, and there was an almost perfect

agreement,

k

= .949 (95% CI, .919 to .978,

p

< .005).

Results

The first aim of this study was to compare the quality of the

HLE between the two groups. The non-parametric Mann

Whitney

U

test was selected due to the small sample size in

each group (

N

= 5). This test is robust against violations to

normality and is not affected by outliers in the data, and is

therefore a suitable choice for this study. A

p

value of <.05

indicated a statistically significant difference between the

groups. It was hypothesised that the HLE of the children

who were D/HH would be less supportive of language

development. This hypothesis was strongly supported (see

Table 3). Five out of seven measures returned a significant

result, with all but one generating a large effect.

The second aim was to examine whether the quantity of

language exposure was different between the groups. The

hypothesised difference in adult word count, conversational

turn count, and child vocalisation count was not supported.

Adult word count in the D/HH group (

Mdn

= 10335, Range

= 4824–19401) was not significantly different to the NH

group (

Mdn

= 7261, Range = 2617–11857),

U

= 10.0,

p

=

.690,

r

= .17. There was a non-significant difference in the

conversational turn count of the D/HH group (

Mdn

= 293,

Range = 134–659) and the NH group (

Mdn

= 409, Range =

247–666),

U

= 12.0,

p

= 1.000,

r

= .03. The child vocalisation

count was also not significantly different in the D/HH (

Mdn

= 1367, Range = 319–2567) and NH (

Mdn

= 1334, Range

= 887-3292) groups,

U

= 12.0,

p

= 1.000,

r

= .03.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate differences in the

quality and quantity of interaction in the HLE between

children who are D/HH and their caregivers, and children

Table 2. Description of intervention received by

D/HH children

Child

Intervention

HI Female 1 Weekly sessions at Telethon Speech and Hearing

since 4/5 months of age.

HI Female 2 Weekly sessions at Telethon Speech and Hearing

since 3 months of age. Articulation therapy from 3

years.

HI Male 3 Twice weekly sessions at Telethon Speech and

Hearing since birth.

HI Male 4 Weekly sessions at Telethon Speech and Hearing

since 3 months of age. Recently changed to

fortnightly sessions.

HI Female 5 Nature of therapy received at Telethon Speech and

Hearing not provided.

Table 3. Descriptive measures, significance, and effect size for quality of interaction measures

Measure

D/HH (

N

= 5)

Median (range)

NH (

N

= 5)

Median (range)

Significance Effect size (

r

)

Number of successful child initiations

17 (13–24)

32 (22–50)

.008**

.76

a

Number of successful caregiver initiations

16 (12–20)

22 (20–28)

.004**

.80

a

Number of connected utterances

44 (27–144)

196 (85–220)

.016*

.69

a

Number of failed utterances

30 (22–42)

22 (13–30)

.075

.50

a

Number of behavioural directives

17 (10–36)

9 (8–28)

.048*

.56

a

Number of caregiver expansions

5 (1–11)

18 (6–31)

.048*

.56

a

Ratio of successful child to caregiver initiations

1.26 (0.75–1.43)

1.28 (1.05–2.45)

.211

.30

b

Note.

All significance levels were one-tailed.

a

large effect

b

medium effect *

p

< .05; **

p

< .01.

Procedure

The Language Environment Analysis (LENA) system is an

audio-recording device used to collect and analyse verbal

information about the HLE (Aragon & Yoshinaga-Itano,

2012). As part of the therapy program at Telethon Speech

and Hearing, caregivers were provided with feedback

surrounding the HLE using the LENA system. The audio

recordings for this group were therefore pre-existing. The

children wore the device continuously for up to 16 hours,

and the caregivers were instructed on the use of the device,

including communicating and interacting naturally. The

caregivers rated how typical the day was on a 5-point Likert

scale in terms of communication, events that occurred, and

overall behaviour. No days were scored at 2 or below.

The audio recordings for both groups were input into

the LENA system, which automatically generated data

for adult word count, conversational turn count, and child

vocalisation count. Two participants did not record for the

entire day, and consequently did not provide a complete

recording. As such, measures were calculated from a

5-hour period (from 9am to 2pm) for all participants to

ensure consistency. The LENA system calculated data

for each of the five hours, and the hour with the median

number of conversational turns was identified. This hour

was coded to provide the conversational quality measures,

thus ensuring a representative sample of the language

environment.