Previous Page  166 / 218 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 166 / 218 Next Page
Page Background

164

Chapter 8

al., 2004; Floresco et al., 2006b; Durstewitz and Seamans, 2008; Stelzel et al., 2010), and was

further strengthened by the observation that pre-treatment with a dopamine D2 receptor

antagonist blocked these effects. Crucially, these effects depended on individual differences

in dopamine signalling, as measured with the

DAT1

genotype: Bromocriptine only improved

task-switching behaviour in subjects homozygous for the 10R allele and did not change task-

switching behaviour in those carrying the 9R allele. Together with the knowledge that the

dopamine transporter is most abundant in the striatum (

chapter 2

), these results suggest that

the stimulation of dopamine D2 receptors

in the striatum

is important for flexible cognitive

control. In addition, these results highlight the importance of taking into account inter-

individual differences in dopamine signalling when assessing drug effects (see

chapter 2

and

(Cools and D’Esposito, 2011). However, genetic associations do not imply causality and a

causal role for dopamine could thus not be provided. Also, when interpreting these results

it is important to keep in mind that expression of the dopamine transporter is not exclusive

to the striatum: The dopamine transporter is also abundantly expressed in the pallidum and

midbrain (Ciliax et al., 1999; Dahlin et al., 2007). In addition, some dopamine transporter

expression is present in the diencephalon, mesencephalon, hippocampus, amygdala and

cortex (Ciliax et al., 1999; Dahlin et al., 2007).

In sum, the results in

chapter 3

replicated previous work suggesting a role for striatal

dopamine in the integration between reward and cognitive control (Aarts et al., 2010).

However, the evidence in

chapter 3

did not support a role for dopamine D2 receptors in

motivated cognitive control. Moreover, the evidence for the involvement of

striatal

dopamine

(i.e. by means of

DAT1

-dependency of the results) is not indisputable. Combining genetics

with neuroimaging (e.g. functional MRI:

box 2.4

and

chapter 4

) can strengthen the evidence

for the involvement of striatal dopamine in motivated cognitive control.

Previous work has suggested a role for dopamine D1 receptor stimulation (Meririnne et al.,

2001), or both dopamine D1 and D2 receptor stimulation (Ikemoto et al., 1997; Koch et al.,

2000) in reward motivation. Methylphenidate (

box 2.2b

) is a drug which blocks the dopamine

transporter, thereby increasing dopamine levels. In

chapter 4

, we manipulated the dopamine

system by using methylphenidate, which is commonly used to pharmacologically treat

ADHD. We assessed patients with ADHD both after intake of their normal dose of Ritalin®

(or an equivalent dose for those usually taking Concerta®;

box 2.2b

) and after refraining from

methylphenidate intake for at least 24 hours. We compared these patients to a healthy control

group to assess cognitive task-related processing as a function of reward-related signalling in

the striatum of adults with ADHD. In this study, we observed that patients with ADHD after

withdrawal from their medication, compared with adults without ADHD, showed increased

neural signalling in the striatum (i.e. in the caudate nucleus) during the integration of reward

and cognitive control. As was the case in

chapter 3

, the effects in

chapter 4

also depended

on natural variation in the

DAT1

genotype: Only the subset of patients carrying the 9R allele

showed this increased striatal activation. Manipulation of the dopamine system, by treatment

of these patients with methylphenidate, normalized this increased striatal signal in the group