Previous Page  4 / 52 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 4 / 52 Next Page
Page Background

Assessment

58

ACQ

Volume 13, Number 2 2011

ACQ

uiring Knowledge in Speech, Language and Hearing

Elise Baker (top)

and Natalie

Munro

This article

has been

peer-

reviewed

Keywords

assessment

children

nonword

repetition

polysyllables

change to assessment practice is based on an assumption

that polysyllables have the potential to (a) provide insight

into the underlying processing difficulties that children with

speech sound disorders (SSD) or specific language

impairment (SLI) might have with the encoding, storage,

and/or retrieval of spoken words (e.g., Contour & McCauley,

2000; Sutherland & Gillon, 2005); (b) help with the

differential diagnosis of late talkers who are at risk of future

speech, language, or literacy difficulties (e.g., Richardson,

Kulju, Neiminen, & Torvelainen, 2009); and (c) improve the

identification of children at risk of future literacy difficulties

who otherwise might be deemed to have typically

developing speech or a mild speech difficulty when

assessed on a measure of consonant accuracy (e.g.,

Nathan & Simpson, 2001). Clinically, the application of this

recommendation to everyday SP practice raises some

fundamental questions with respect to age of assessment.

Specifically, at what age should and/or could children’s

pronunciation of polysyllables be reliably assessed? Should

SPs wait until children are of preschool or school age to

assess production of polysyllables? Is it better or indeed

possible to evaluate children’s productions of polysyllables

during the toddler years (between the age of approximately

1;0 to 3;0 years)?

According to James, van Doorn, and McLeod (2008),

children’s acquisition of polysyllabic words is gradual and

protracted, with refinement of syllable timing continuing

into adolescence. This does not mean, however, that

young children do not produce polysyllabic words. Children

with typical development have been observed to produce

polysyllables in their first 50 words (e.g., Savinainen-

Makkonen, 2000). Although children’s initial attempts may

be truncated (e.g., helicopter /

hɛlikɒptə

/ as [

kɒtə

]) (Kehoe &

Stoel-Gammon, 1997), the number of polysyllables in which

all syllables are represented (rather than deleted) changes

from 0% to about 50% by 2;3 years (James, 2006). Clearly,

if routine assessment of children’s pronunciations of

polysyllables is to be conducted, it would seem appropriate

to begin that evaluation with children from the time they

start to talk – during the toddler years. The purpose of this

paper is to explore the literature on the potential clinical

value of assessing toddlers’ productions of polysyllables

(both real words and imitated nonwords), and to review

currently available experimental tasks and clinical resources

for assessing Australian-English-speaking toddlers’

productions of polysyllabic real- and nonwords.

Historically, routine assessment of children’s

speech has focused on consonant accuracy

(e.g., ability to pronounce /k/ in

car

,

bucket

,

and

bike

). The discovery of a link between the

ability to produce polysyllables and speech,

language, phonological processing, and later

literacy abilities suggests that speech

pathologists (SPs) need to extend their focus

from consonant accuracy to children’s ability

to produce polysyllables, considering syllable

number, shape, and stress pattern accuracy.

This paper reviews a range of experimental

tasks and clinical tools that SPs could use to

examine toddlers’ productions of polysyllabic

real- and nonwords. Given that assessment

of toddlers’ productions of polysyllables is a

relatively new area of research, SPs are

encouraged to assess toddlers’ polysyllable

productions within the context of a

comprehensive communication

assessment.

A

s children learn to speak, they not only learn how

to articulate the individual consonants and vowels

in their ambient language, but also to pronounce

words of varying syllable shapes, word lengths, and stress

patterns. Historically, routine assessment of children’s

speech has focused on their ability to accurately articulate

consonants in initial, medial, and final word positions,

typically in mono– and/or disyllabic words (e.g.,

pronunciation of /k/ in

car

,

bucket

, and

bike

). The discovery

of a link between the ability to produce polysyllables (words

of three or more syllables) and speech, language,

phonological processing, and later literacy abilities (e.g.,

Dollaghan & Campbell, 1998; Sutherland & Gillon, 2005),

suggests that speech pathologists (SPs) need to extend

their focus from consonant accuracy to one that considers

children’s abilities to produce polysyllables, including their

ability to match syllable number, syllable shape, and stress

pattern in real words such as

helicopter

and

spaghetti

, and

in nonwords such as /

pɜduləmeɪp

/ and /

doʊpəlut

/ (from

Stokes & Klee, 2009b). The impetus for this recommended

An overview of resources

for assessing toddlers’

productions of polysyllables

Elise Baker and Natalie Munro