Table of Contents Table of Contents
Previous Page  38 / 100 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 38 / 100 Next Page
Page Background

37

JCPSLP

Volume 18, Number 1 2016

Journal of Clinical Practice in Speech-Language Pathology

participants’ scores on the rhyme awareness subtest of

the PIPA compared with the other subtests (Wilks’ Lamda

= .686, F(4, 26) = 2.98, p = .038, multivariate partial eta

squared = .314), indicating that, on average, children with

CL/P in this study presented with poorer phonological

awareness skills in rhyme awareness.

The mean language score for this sample, based on the

CELF-P2 or CELF-4 assessment, was 94.50 (SD = 13.84;

range = 61–118), indicating that, on average, children with

CL/P in this study presented with language skills within

the average range. Participants’ language scores were

highly variable however, with 8 children (27%) in the study

presenting with a standard scores below 85.

This study also aimed to investigate the nature of the

relationship between phonological awareness abilities in

5-year-old children with CL/P and language skills, speech

production, middle ear function, and velopharyngeal

function. Table 3 illustrates correlations, and associated

levels of significance, between the dependent variable,

phonological awareness, and each independent variable.

A marginally significant correlation was detected between

phonological awareness scores and language scores (R

2

= .355, p = .054). Paired t-tests revealed no statistically

significant association between phonological awareness

and speech diagnosis (.060, p = .754), velopharyngeal

function (.028, p = .883), and middle ear function (–.044, p

= .817). Further, no statistically significant association was

detected between phonological awareness scores and

gender (–.124, p = .514), and cleft type (.167, p = .377).

Given no statistically significant correlation between

phonological awareness and speech diagnosis,

velopharyngeal function, and middle ear function was

detected, these four variables were omitted from the

final regression model. Table 4 displays the results of the

final univariate regression. A histogram of the residuals

closely approximated a normal distribution, validating

the assumption of normality for error in the model. The

final model accounted for 12.6% (p = .054) of variance in

phonological awareness scores. Language Score (.328,

p = .054) had a marginally significant relationship with

phonological awareness skills, indicating that language

abilities may influence the phonological awareness skills of

children with CL/P.

Discussion

The aims of this exploratory study were to explore

phonological awareness skills in 5-year-old children with

conducted using paired t-tests (for continuous variables)

and Pearson correlations (for ordinal variables) to determine

multicollinearity between the variables. The significance level

was set at

.05. Bivariate analysis revealed no significant

relationships between phonological awareness and speech

production, velopharyngeal function, and middle ear

function. Univariate regression was then used to explore

the relationship between phonological awareness skills and

language performance. Regression analysis was used as a

statistical process for estimating the relationship between

phonological awareness and language performance. Model

significance was analysed using overall regression ANOVA.

Coefficient significance levels were set at

.05.

Results

Table 1 displays a summary of speech diagnosis,

velopharyngeal function, and middle ear function of the 30

participants in the study. These assessment results suggest

that the majority of children with CL/P in this study

presented with adequate velopharyngeal function (n = 20;

66.7%), poor speech production skills (n = 24; 80%), and

abnormal middle ear function (n = 25; 73.3%).

The mean composite phonological awareness score for

this sample based on standard scores of the five subtests

of the PIPA was 44.50 (SD = 12.77; range = 25–71). Table

2 presents the descriptive statistics of each subtest of the

PIPA. According to test norms, mean standard scores

on each of the PIPA subtests were within the average

range (i.e., 7–13). However, participants’ scores were

highly variable. There was a significant difference between

Table 2. Participant’s standard scores on individual

subtests of the PIPA (N = 30)

PIPA subtest

M SD Range

Syllable Segmentation

9.23 3.059 3–15

Rhyme Awareness

7.43 3.014 3–13

Alliteration Awareness

9.23 3.081 4–15

Phoneme Isolation

9.40 4.123 6–16

Phoneme Segmentation

9.20 5.492 3–17

N.B.Standardscoresbetween7and13indicateskillswithinnormal

limits

Table 3. Summary of correlations between phonological awareness and candidate variables for regression

model (N = 30)

Variable

Language

score

Speech

diagnosis

Velopharyngeal

function

Middle ear

function

Gender

Cleft type

Correlation Phonologicalawareness

.335

.060

.028

–.044

–.124

.167

Significance

.054

.754

.883

.817

.514

.377

Table 4. Summary of final regression model (N = 30)

Model

Standardised co-efficients

t-value Significance 95.0% confidence interval for – Correlations

Beta

Lower bound Upperbound

Part

Constant

.869

.392

–18.360

45.392

Language score

.328

2.012

.054

–.006

.662

.355