Background Image
Table of Contents Table of Contents
Previous Page  25 / 64 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 25 / 64 Next Page
Page Background www.speechpathologyaustralia.org.au

JCPSLP

Volume 17, Number 3 2015

135

In a final example, Romski and colleagues (2010)

investigated the impact of speech generating devices

(SGDs) during language interventions. They assigned

groups of children, aged 2–3 years old, to three

experimental conditions: speech only; aided input (where

participants were prompted to communicate using a

mid-tech voice output system); and aided output (where

communication partners modelled the use of a mid-tech

voice output system). The children who received the two

aided interventions produced significantly larger amounts of

vocabulary (either via speech or their AAC system) than the

speech only group (Romski et al., 2010).

Timing of AAC implementation

Experts in the field of AAC recommend that AAC is

introduced as early as possible in order to avoid potential

long-term negative outcomes associated with

communication impairment (Cress & Marvin, 2003; Drager

et al., 2010; Light & Drager, 2007; Romski & Sevcik, 2005;

Van Tatenhove, 1987). As Romski and Sevcik stated: “AAC

is not a last resort but rather a first line of intervention that

can provide a firm foundation for the development of

spoken language comprehension and production” (p. 183).

Cress and Marvin (2003) suggested that AAC intervention

should commence if a child’s communication is difficult for

a child’s caregivers or communication partners to decipher.

Often one of the reasons for delaying AAC

implementation with young children is that the majority of

basic wants and needs are generally well interpreted and

adequately met by a child’s primary caregiver. The risk in

this approach however, is that it teaches young children

to become dependent on informed and familiar listeners.

This may lead to the development of learned helplessness

where the child becomes used to not being able to

communicate effectively and ceases to have the motivation

to continue trying (Van Tatenhove, 1987). Suggestions

for preventing learned helplessness involve coaching

communication partners to recognise and support early

communication attempts, and demonstrating the power

of communication through the use of “core vocabulary”

in motivating and functional contexts (Judge et al., 2010;

Olive, Lang, & Davis, 2008; Romski & Sevcik, 2005; Van

Tatenhove, 1987).

Core vocabulary is a set of 400–500 words that feature

in approximately 80% of the speech people produce

(Banajee, Dicarlo, & Stricklin, 2003; Cannon & Edmond,

2009; Clendon & Erickson, 2008; Clendon, Sturm, & Cali,

2013; Trembath, Balandin, & Togher, 2007). They include

words such as:

more

,

again

,

stop

,

go

,

finished

,

this

,

that

,

etc. These words have the ability to hold power over a

communication partner by directing an interaction. They

also have high utility and thus provide lots of opportunities

for modelling and repeated use (Geist, Hatch, & Erickson,

2014). The remainder of words that make up a person’s

vocabulary (usually many thousands of words) are referred

to as “fringe” or “extended vocabulary”.

A further factor influencing the timing of AAC

implementation is the reality that many children with CCN

have compromised health status. As a consequence,

often therapies which are perceived as additional or

optional are held back until the child’s health is more

stable. This can be a dangerous approach when it comes

to AAC implementation as without a functional means of

communication, these children run the risk of being unable

to express themselves effectively (Cress & Marvin, 2003).

It is critical to acknowledge, however, that parents of

children with CCN are frequently dealing with considerable

discussed the substantial benefits inherent in access to

these technologies but also highlighted the increased

demands they place on linguistic, operational, and social

competence. They stated that:

with the dramatic change in the scope of

communication and the explosion of tools through

which to meet communication needs, individuals with

complex communication needs now have access

potentially to a much wider and more diverse audience

than ever before. (Light & McNaughton, 2014, p. 9)

This means, however, that the communication expectations

for AAC users have changed. They must develop the skills

required to “independently use these new tools, adhere to

their conventions, and communicate with a broader

audience that includes those who may have limited or no

prior experience with AAC” (p. 9).

Challenging myths and

misconceptions

Despite the increased availability and implementation of

AAC within some communities, there continue to be myths

and misconceptions about the appropriateness and timing

of AAC intervention with infants and young children. A

number of highly regarded experts in the field of AAC have

written about these issues (Cress & Marvin, 2003; Judge,

Floyd, & Wood-Fields, 2010; Romski & Sevcik, 2005; Van

Tatenhove, 1987).

AAC and speech development

One common assumption is that AAC use will impede or

prevent speech development (Van Tatenhove, 1987). It is

well documented that these concerns are unwarranted

(Cress & Marvin, 2003; Johnston, McDonnell, Nelson, &

Magnavito, 2003; Romski et al., 2010; Stahmer & Ingersoll,

2004). Research studies have demonstrated that AAC

intervention for children below 5 years of age assists with

the development of speech, language and functional

communication skills (e.g., Drager et al., 2010; Johnston et

al., 2003; Dunst, Trivette, Hamby, & Simkus, 2013; Romski

et al., 2010; Stahmer & Ingersoll, 2004).

Johnston and colleagues (2003), for example, introduced

no-tech (Picture Communication Symbols [PCS]) and low-

tech (single message voice output device) AAC systems

to two children (one aged 3;10 with developmental delay

and the other aged 4;6 with athetoid cerebral palsy

and developmental delay) across an average of four

sessions. Their intervention strategies included (a) creating

appropriate and motivating communication opportunities;

(b) modelling use of the AAC system by peers and

teachers; (c) least to most prompting, and (d) naturally

occurring consequences for communication attempts. The

children demonstrated a 100% increase in the correct use

of unprompted symbolic communication (Johnston et al.,

2003).

In another study, Stahmer and Ingersoll (2014) explored

the effectiveness of an EI service for children with autism

spectrum disorder (ASD). The Picture Exchange

Communication System (PECS) and modified sign language

were implemented simultaneously with students who were

described as nonverbal. Not only did the use of AAC appear

to assist spoken language development (80% exited the

program with spoken language), but by the completion of

the intervention, 90% of participants were able to

independently use a functional communication system

compared to only 50% when the study commenced.