Previous Page  110 / 218 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 110 / 218 Next Page
Page Background

108

Chapter 5

effect on switch trials (Age x Reward: r (ρ) = - 0.367, p < 0.001;

figure 5.2b

). Inspection of

figure 5.2b

shows that these correlations were driven by Reward effects in young, but not old

participants. Young, but not old participants, showed a reward-related increase in speed-over-

accuracy on switch trials, and a reward-related decrease in speed-over-accuracy on repeat

trials. Thus, the main task effects, observed across the group as a whole and described above,

were driven by younger participants and were absent in older participants.

The degree to which a promised reward affected task switching did not change with age in

terms of response times (Age x Reward x Task switching: r (ρ) = -0.038, p = 0.714) or accuracy

(Age x Reward x Task switching: r (ρ) = -0.178, p = 0.053).

We validated the SAT effects in a subgroup of participants who all received the same reward

size (i.e. group A, C and D,

table 5.1

). Age-dependent effects in this subsample, which was

not confounded by differential reward size, resembled those observed in the large sample. A

negative correlation was observed between Age and the effect of Reward on Task switching

(Age x Reward x Task switching: r (ρ) = - 0.360, p = 0.003;

figure 5.3a

). This three-way

interaction was again due to a positive correlation between Age and the Reward effect on

repeat trials (Age x Reward: r (ρ) = 0.277, p = 0.022) and a negative correlation between Age

and the Reward effect on switch trials (Age x Reward: r (ρ) = -0.373, p = 0.002). The result

from this continuous analysis with Age as a covariate was confirmed by a between-group

independent samples Mann-Whitney U test with Age as a between-subject factor. For this

analysis, the subgroup (N = 68, mean age 30.29, range 14 – 67 years) was split into two groups

based on the median age (25 years old; youngest group: N = 35, mean 16.23 (SE 0.48) years

old; oldest group: N = 33, mean 45.18 (SE 2.29) years old; U = 7.114, p < 0.001). A significant

Age x Reward x Task-switching interaction was revealed (U = -2.755, p = 0.006), which was

due to a Reward x Task switching interaction in the younger group (W = -3.849, p < 0.001),

but not in the older group (W = -1.885, p = 0.059) (

table 5.2, figure 5.3b

).

In sum, aging was accompanied by diminished effects of Reward on Task switching in terms

of SAT. This effect was confirmed in a smaller subsample, corrected for reward size.

Age-related changes in response deadlines and earned rewards

We hypothesized that age-related changes in task switching would be grounded in

motivational changes. Increasing age was indeed associated with cognitive changes: We

observed age-related changes in task switching. In addition, we observed smaller reward

effects with age, both across repeat and switch trials and as a function of task switching. In a

supplementary analysis, we assessed whether this reward-related deficit in terms of behavior

was accompanied by changes in total earnings.

Surprisingly, we observed an age-related

increase

in the total reward earned on the rewarded

task-switching paradigm. This effect was observed in the large sample (n=118) (Age x

Total reward: r (ρ) = 0.581, p < 0.001), and in the subgroup of 68 participants in which the

maximum bonus did not vary across participants (Age x Total reward: r (ρ) = 0.309, p =