Previous Page  111 / 218 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 111 / 218 Next Page
Page Background

109

Reward modulation of cognitive function: aging

0.010). A between subject analysis in this subgroup revealed that the older group earned more

reward than did the younger group (mean €9.01 (SE 0.32) vs. mean €10.50 (SE 0.20); t(26.448)

= 11.343, p < 0.001).

We were puzzled by this effect and reasoned that the age-related increase in total earnings

might originate from differences in the response deadline, which was set during a pre-test

practice phase (

methods

). When the response deadline was determined, participants were

instructed to respond as fast and accurately as possible. We reasoned that participants who

put more emphasis on the accuracy instruction would not respond as fast as possible during

practice. This would then result in longer, less stringent response deadlines during the actual

test. In the current paradigm, inaccurate responses, no matter how fast, are never rewarded.

Therefore, adopting such a cautious (slow and accurate) response strategy during practice

may result in higher earnings. For example, imagine two participants (A and B) who are

theoretically both able to respond within 400ms. If participant

A

responds cautiously during

the practice phase, the average response time during practice will be slower (e.g. 900ms)

than that of someone who emphasized speed during practice (participant B, e.g. 500ms). As

a consequence, participant A will have plenty of time to respond accurately on test, thereby

increasing the number of rewarded trials. By contrast, participant B will need to continue

to respond relatively fast. Participant B will thus make more errors, and therefore a lower

number of trials will be rewarded.

To test the idea that the response strategy during practice differed with age and that this would

lead to the observed age-related differences in earnings, we first assessed whether age was

associated with the length of the individually determined response deadlines. We observed

an overall age-related increase in the response deadline (i.e. across 4 trial-types: Arrow/Word

x Switch/Repeat), so that older participants were allowed to respond more slowly on test than

did younger participants (Age x Response deadline: (ρ) = 0.587, p <0.001). One might argue

that the differential Age x Reward effects on repeat and switch trials reported above might

Table 5.2

Reward x Task-switching effects for younger and older

subgroups*

Younger

(n=35)

Older

(n=33)

Difference

RT

-0.164

(-10.50;10.17)

-0.486

(-9.58;8.61)

-0.055;

P > 0.1

Accuracy

0.66

(-3.24;4.56)

-2.21

(-5.70;1.27)

-1.061;

P > 0.1

SAT

0.43

(0.25;0.61)

0.15

(0.01;0.29)

-2.755;

p = 0.006

SAT = Speed-Accuracy-Tradeoff = (z-speed- z-accuracy) / 2 ; RT = response times

* subgroups (study A, C and D in table 1 and figure 3a) were not confounded by

differential reward size