Previous Page  114 / 218 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 114 / 218 Next Page
Page Background

112

Chapter 5

accompanied by reward-based changes in flexible cognitive control, where older participants

no longer exhibit an effect of reward motivation on task switching performance.

Reward-induced changes in the speed-accuracy tradeoff have not been widely studied, and

never in the context of a cognitive task. However, at least one study suggests that potential

rewards can induce more cautious (i.e. more accurate and slow) behavior (Bijleveld et al.,

2010). We replicate this effect in the cognitively less demanding repeat trials by showing that

young participants exhibit reward-induced cautious behavior in the repeat condition. We

extend this finding by showing that reward induced less accurate and faster responding on

the more demanding switch trials in younger participants. These results suggest that reward

can modulate cautious behavior differentially in distinct cognitive conditions.

Reward motivation, cognitive control and the interaction between reward and cognitive

control implicate, among other mechanisms, dopamine in the striatum (Roberts et al., 1994;

Ikemoto and Panksepp, 1999; Aarts et al., 2010; Aarts et al., 2011). Furthermore, changes in

speed-accuracy tradeoff strategies have been associated with changes in connectivity between

the cortex and the striatum (Bogacz et al., 2010). More specifically, stronger corticostriatal

connections have been found to promote faster (and premature) responses. The current

study does not address the neural mechanisms of age-related effects on rewarded task

switching directly. However, several independent studies have revealed age-related changes

in dopamine signaling (Volkow et al., 1996b; Bäckman et al., 2000) Erixon-Lindroth et al.,

2005), starting in early adulthood (Backman and Farde, 2005). Also, age-related decreases in

corticostriatal connectivity, accompanied by slower (and more accurate) responses have been

reported (Forstmann et al., 2011). Accordingly, here, we put forward the hypothesis that the

observed change in speed-accuracy strategy during the integration of reward and cognitive

performance reflects reduced dopamine signaling in the striatum, and a subsequent reduction

in the adaptation of corticostriatal responses to the task conditions. The increased effect of

reward in younger versus older participants is also in line with a number of previous findings

in adolescents. First, neuroimaging work has revealed increases in reward sensitivity and

ventral striatal responses in adolescents (Somerville and Casey, 2010). In addition, previous

work has reported reward-related improvements in impulse control in adolescents (Kohls et

al., 2009; Geier et al., 2010). In older participants, however, if anything a decrease in reward

sensitivity is reported (Schott et al., 2007; Rademacher et al., 2014). We extend these findings

by showing that young (i.e. < 26 years old) subjects can show reward-related adaptations of

flexible cognitive control during task switching, whereas older participants (i.e. > 25 years

old) cannot.

One limitation of the current study is the fact that we pooled data from several studies. As

a result, there are a number of factors of no interest that differ as a function of age, such as

effects of maximum available reward. However, we accounted for this by replicating the effects

in a subgroup of participants who received exactly the same amount of reward and number of

trials. Nevertheless, it is clear that the reported results require replication in future, preferably

longitudinal, work using a single study set-up. Also, it should be noted that a large number of