Previous Page  115 / 218 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 115 / 218 Next Page
Page Background

113

Reward modulation of cognitive function: aging

methods exist which can be used to assess speed accuracy tradeoffs (Salthouse and Hedden,

2002; Forstmann et al., 2011; Heitz, 2014). The method used in this study is an elementary

formalization of a speed accuracy tradeoff. Nevertheless, the results clearly show that taking

into account both speed and accuracy can reveal differences that would not be revealed when

response times and accuracy are assessed separately. However, future work should extend these

findings using more sophisticated approaches (e.g. mathematical decision making models)

and by experimentally manipulating speed accuracy strategies (Heitz, 2014). Such model-

based approaches might be more sensitive, as they take into account trial-by-trial changes

in speed and accuracy. The consequence of the simple composite score approach used in the

current study is that changes in speed and accuracy are assumed to contribute equally to the

decision process. It is therefore hard to interpret the SAT measure used in the current study

without taking note of the separate response and accuracy measures. Finally, we cannot rule

out completely that older participants show reduced motivational effects because they value

money less than young participants. However, we argue that this is unlikely for two reasons.

First, age was associated with contrasting effects of reward on switch and repeat trials. Second,

older participants actually earned more money than did the younger participants.

In the current study we controlled for general age-related differences in processing speed

(Salthouse, 1996) by determining each individual’s response deadline, by using a within-

subject comparison of conditions, and by taking into account the speed-accuracy tradeoff.

In line with previous work (Bijleveld et al., 2010; Forstmann et al., 2011), older participants

were more cautious (i.e. slower and more accurate than younger participants). This cautious

strategy was already evident during practice and allowed older participants more time to

respond accurately during test. It is unlikely that allowing slower (older) participants more

time has induced differences in reward-related speed-accuracy strategies, given the equal

response deadline for high and low reward (i.e. only arrow/word x switch/repeat trials were

adapted). In addition, the response deadline adaptations did not result in an age-related

difference in switch and repeat trials, suggesting it is unlikely that this adaptation has changed

the accuracy-over-speed strategy during the integration of reward and task switching

differently in older and younger participants. The cautiousness of the older participants was

further corroborated by the observation that the overall response deadline of the younger

participants was closer to their maximum performance in terms of speed (on test). Crucially

however, this difference was not related to the age-related adaptation to the task conditions.

Here, we show for the first time that age-related changes in response strategies can be observed

when participants need to flexibly adapt to changing task demands. Specifically, we observed

an age-related decrease in the degree to which older participants use information about

rewards to change their speed-accuracy strategy in changing cognitive control conditions;

i.e., older participants no longer used rewards to adapt cognitive control processes. As such

the present study goes beyond prior work focusing commonly merely on cognitive deficits

(Salthouse, 1996; West, 1996; Kray et al., 2002; Park et al., 2002). An obvious next step would

be to unravel the neural mechanisms underlying these changes.