Previous Page  42 / 88 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 42 / 88 Next Page
Page Background

Reading Matters

Teaching Matters

|

40

|

Reading Matters | Volume 16 • Winter 2016 |

scira.org CLICK HERE TO RETURN TO TABLE OF CONTENTS

encouraged her to learn how to hold the book and turn the

page and discover when the next section of print was coming

up. Observing how these tools were effective in teaching Sarah

prompted Julie’s interest in learning more about how songs,

chants, and sing-alongs can promote literacy in early readers.

Anvari, Trainor, Woodside, and Levy (2002) emphasize the

relationship between music and reading acquisition, particularly

in relation to phonemic and phonological awareness. To clarify,

phonemic awareness is a type of phonological awareness and

refers to discernment of phonemes. Phonological awareness

encompasses “

any size unit of sound

” (Yopp & Yopp, 2000, p. 130);

the ability to recognize and say rhyming words, to count syllables,

to segment word parts such as the beginning

/ch/

and ending

/ip/

are examples of such awareness. Early experiences with language,

especially speech that is intuitively altered when directed at

young infants, involve musical attributes. These attributes include

repetition, tempo or pacing, up/down patterns in pitch, much

like the rising, falling, drawn out, and staccato notes in a song.

Children hear and become sensitized to these differences. This

parallel between music and early speech suggests that “early

skill with music might enhance reading acquisition to the extent

that reading depends on the same basic auditory analysis skills”

(Anvari et al., 2002, p. 113). What is most important here is that

home and school experiences build sensitivity to the sounds of

spoken language. Yopp and Yopp (2000) suggest several guidelines

for activities that promote such sensitivity and awareness: (a)

playfulness; (b) intentionality in focusing on the sounds of spoken

language; and (c) part of a comprehensive reading framework.

Combining such experiences with music is likely to incorporate

these elements while being memorable and engaging.

Phonological awareness is more likely to develop through

repetition, pronunciation, and rhyming patterns within songs and

chants. Hearing individual sounds within words and associating

phonemes with specific letters can be supported through

singing and listening to songs. Hearing the pronunciation of

words modeled as syllables as they are elongated in song may be

beneficial to readers, especially if they are following along with

the text in front of them (Anvari et al., 2002). For example, The

Animal Boogie

(Harter, 2005) video

https://www.youtube.com/ watch?v=25_u1GzruQM

includes many examples of repetition and

stretching letter sounds within its verses. Vowel sounds like the

oo vowel team in “boogie” are repeated in the chorus of “boogie

woogie oogie.”While examples such as these may seem silly or

simply playful to adults, children love playing with the sounds and

do not realize that they are also building necessary understandings

for early literacy development as they are singing. The singer

in the video stretches out consonant sounds such as the

/l/

in

leopard and the slithering sound of the

/s/

in snake. Emphasizing

these sounds while singing and tracking along with the print in

the book makes reading appeal to the auditory senses while also

accentuating phonemic awareness and letter-sound concepts.

When song picture books are used, concepts about print are

more meaningful, and print conventions are learned in context

(Fisher, 2001; Paquette and Rieg, 2008). For instance, songs with

patterns can be used to support print concepts such as one-to-

one or voice-print matching. To clarify, we might start with a fairly

simple text with single-syllable words such as

The Wheels on the Bus

(Kovalski, 1987; Zelinsky, 1990), as it is a song selection the child

already knows. Because the child has memory for the words (and

tune), he is more likely to match his voice to the single-syllable

words on the page as he follows along with his finger. Thus, he is

freed up to focus on regulating the voice-print matching, rather

than having to also decode the words. As his print concepts

mature, the learner is ready for the next step. We might choose

to sing and read,

Over in the Meadow

(Galdone, 1986). Once the

child is familiar with the words to the song, the text can serve as a

self-tutorial for the child, helping him to learn to regulate voice-

print matching or pointing behavior with multi-syllabic words.

As he follows along with the words, he is likely to recognize that

his finger needs to stay on the word, meadow, for two beats.

“Over in the meadow

in the sand in the sun

Lived an old mother turtle

and her little turtle one.”

As an added resource, children can watch videos such as

Over in

the Meadow

( https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=C6ljGXMMB-g )

and read and sing along. A number of these videos function similarly

to an e-storybook with highlighted words and animated dots that

track the print for the child. Of course, most children will eventually

develop the phonological awareness to understanding that words

like meadow have two parts or two claps while bus has one, but the

joint access to music and text is likely to speed up such awareness.

Research with e-books and CD-ROMs suggest that incorporation

of animations and sound as opposed to static visuals (printed texts

and illustrations) are likely to assist the development of reading

skills, especially in children who are at-risk (Neuman, 2009; Shamir

& Shlafer, 2011). Such multimedia learning environments, when

high quality, purposeful, and coordinated, offer multiple entries for

acquiring literacy as opposed to a single visual print medium (Mayer,

2003). In cases where videos, CDs, or e-books or even picturebooks

are unavailable, singing while reading teacher/class-made charts

or Big Books offers a multi-sensory substitute. Such created texts,

in lieu of, or in addition to, these more costly resources are likely

to inspire pride in creativity and reading-writing connections.

To support word learning in reading and writing, children

can also create their own variants of familiar songs. To provide

demonstrations, teachers might use published variations of

a familiar song such as

Over at the Castle

(Ashburn, 2010) or

Berkes’

Over in the Arctic

(2008) or

Over in Australia

(2011) to

suggest the playful transformation of

Over in the Meadow

. The

latter two teach about different types of animals and their

habitats but also serve as mentor texts, demonstrating how

to create a variant. As an early example, teachers might use a

counting song,

Five Little Ducks

(Raffi, 1989) to read and sing:

“Five little ducks went out one day

over the hill and far away.

Mother duck said,

‘Quack, quack, quack, quack.’

But only four little ducks came back.”