Table of Contents Table of Contents
Previous Page  5 / 100 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 5 / 100 Next Page
Page Background www.speechpathologyaustralia.org.au

JCPSLP

Volume 18, Number 1 2016

4

Methods

Study 1

The data from the first study are derived from a larger study,

carried out in Western Australia in 2011, that explored

SLPs’ accounts and perceptions of their management

decisions for people with aphasia in acute and rehabilitation

settings. SLPs used a workbook, developed for the study,

to record information about their management of a single

person with aphasia (PWA) on their caseload. The SLPs

recorded information about the individual’s medical history,

the SLP goals they had for the individual, their intentions

regarding the frequency with which they planned to see the

person and information on each occasion of SLP service

provided to the individual while they were on the SLPs’

caseload. The SLPs were asked to complete the workbook

for the first person diagnosed with aphasia who was

admitted to their caseload after they had consented to

participate in the study. Upon completion of the workbook,

the SLPs took part in an interview to confirm the details

recorded in the workbook and explore points raised

regarding their clinical decision making for the specific

PWA. The interviews were transcribed verbatim.

The current study reports on a subset of data drawn from

the experiences of the eight SLPs who worked in acute

hospital settings and is focused on their decision making

around the goals they developed for their in-patients

and the level of service delivery (length and frequency of

sessions) they planned to provide for the individual with

aphasia. The data is drawn from both the workbooks and

the follow up interviews, analysed using thematic analysis

(Braun & Clarke, 2006). A portion of the workbook data has

been reported previously in Ciccone, Hersh, Armstrong,

and Godecke (2013). Ethics approval for this research was

granted through the Edith Cowan University HREC and the

HRECs of the hospital involved in the study.

Study 2

The second study (Hersh, 2003) was based on interview

data with 30 SLPs collected as part of a doctoral study,

analysed using a grounded theory methodology (Strauss &

Corbin, 1998). Ethics approval for this research was

granted through the Ethics Committee of Flinders Medical

Centre, the University’s board for clinical applications in the

School of Medicine, and the ethics committees of other

employing institutions (three other hospitals, a rehabilitation

centre, and two community health centres). One aspect of

the interviews involved exploring the factors that clinicians

considered influenced their decisions to keep patients or

clients on in therapy or to discharge them. While a range of

factors influenced decision-making, data reported for this

current paper refers only to one ‘patient-specific factor’,

that of motivation. All names used below from both studies

are pseudonyms.

Results

Even though the data from these two studies were

collected across states, nearly a decade apart by different

researchers, and focused on clinical decisions across

acute, rehabilitation and community settings, there were

overlapping findings and themes (see Table 1). Motivation

was considered as one of a cluster of factors influencing

decisions, for example, relating to client, service-level, or

wider contextual considerations. Examples of other

client-level factors reported by Hersh (2003) were: age,

severity of aphasia, health status, time post-onset,

premorbid and current communication needs, language

role of motivation in stroke rehabilitation more broadly.

Motivation is actually a poorly defined notion (Maclean &

Pound, 2000), but it is often cited by clinicians as a critical

factor in determining the outcome of stroke rehabilitation

generally (Becker & Kaufman, 1995; Gold, 1983; Lewinter

& Mikkelsen, 1995; Maclean & Pound, 2000). This has also

been reflected in some aphasia-related research (Lendrem,

1994; Mackenzie et al., 1993). Becker and Kaufman

(1995) reported that judgments of patient motivation were

a key indicator of rehabilitation potential in the eyes of

stroke clinicians. Maclean and Pound (2000) reached a

similar conclusion. They highlighted the dangers of seeing

motivation only as a personal trait and then “moralising”

about worthiness and character in those where motivation

was judged to be lacking. They cited a number of studies

that suggested that: “…a moralistic approach within

rehabilitation settings can have deleterious effects on

patient care, and also on the quality of patients’ lives after

discharge” (p. 503). In a study involving interviews with

32 members of a multidisciplinary team (although only

including 2 SLPs) Maclean, Pound, Wolfe, and Rudd (2002)

found that:

The criteria professionals use to recognize motivation

have been shown to have blurred boundaries.

“Motivated” patients are expected to be proactive, but

this proactivity must never manifest itself in a strong-

willed rejection of therapy. Similarly, motivated patients

are expected to be compliant, but this compliance

must never be the total compliance associated with

a lack of “intrinsic”motivation. In effect, patients walk

a fine line regarding how their behavior is viewed; a

delicate balance has to be struck between compliance

and proactivity if the patient is to avoid being seen

as unmotivated and therefore receiving a potentially

damaging label. (p. 448)

Professional expectations about motivation can become

blinkered, for example, varying with the age of the patient

(Nicholas, Rybarczyk, Meyer, Lacey, Haut, & Kemp, 1998)

and can be insensitive to the sheer effort patients are

expected to make in order to be regarded as motivated.

Meier and Purtillo (1994) warned against labelling people

as “poorly motivated”when they “may not understand

the effort, pain, repetition, boredom and altered body use

that is required” (p. 365). Finally, adding further to this

complexity is the issue of post stroke fatigue. This may

occur with depression but certainly does not have to, and

therefore may not respond to antidepressant medication. It

is estimated to occur in around 40% of people post stroke

(Lynch et al., 2007) and may also interfere with people’s

ability to manage the work demanded in rehabilitation

(Morley, Jackson, & Mead, 2005). The presence of fatigue

has obvious implications for the way in which clinicians

predict rehabilitation potential, and it is not difficult to

imagine that patients suffering from post-stroke fatigue

could be viewed as too poorly motivated to participate in

therapy.

In the light of this review, and the particular issues facing

people with aphasia, we suggest that the issue of how

judgments of motivation might impact on predictions of

potential in aphasia rehabilitation deserve more attention.

In this paper, we draw on original data from two separate

studies carried out by each of the authors to highlight

examples of how judgments of motivation by SLPs

influence decision-making and management of people with

aphasia in post-stroke rehabilitation.