Table of Contents Table of Contents
Previous Page  7 / 100 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 7 / 100 Next Page
Page Background www.speechpathologyaustralia.org.au

JCPSLP

Volume 18, Number 1 2016

6

unmotivated. Our participants in this study have judged

such a lack of motivation through a range of behaviours

including non-attendance, passivity, or not completing

home practice. We need to recognize that judgments of

motivation may perhaps be about behaviours which reflect

patient satisfaction with therapy – although within a

particularly complex context of post-stroke rehabilitation.

SLPs need to step back and consider how judgments of

motivation might be impacting on their practice. We

suggest that, in the context of aphasia rehabilitation, this is

particularly crucial because language impairments can have

such a profound impact on patients’ ability to make sense

of therapy and of rehabilitation as a whole and, along with

all the other variables discussed in this paper, could impact

on how motivated they appear to be. Aphasia rehabilitation

services are often time and resource limited – so there is

certainly pressure to direct such resources towards the

most benefit. However, how to make decisions regarding

the potential to benefit from therapy at an individual level

remains a difficult issue – but one which would be assisted

by further discussion and reflection.

References

Becker, G., & Kaufman, S. (1995). Managing an uncertain

illness trajectory in old age: Patients’ and physicians’ views

of stroke. Medical Anthropology Quarterly, 9, 165–187.

Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis

in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(2),

77–101.

Bright, F.A.S., Kayes, N.M., Worrall, L., & McPherson,

K.M. (2015). A conceptual review of engagement in

healthcare and rehabilitation. Disability and Rehabilitation,

37(8), 643–654.

Burton, C.R., Horne, M., Woodward-Nutt, K., Bowen,

A., & Tyrrell, P. (2015). What is rehabilitation potential?

Development of a theoretical model through the accounts

of healthcare professionals working in stroke rehabilitation

services. Disability and Rehabilitation, 37(21), 1955–1960.

DOI: 10.3109/09638288.2014.991454

Ciccone, N., Armstrong, E., Hersh, D., & Godecke, E.

(2013). Speech pathologists’ clinical decision making in the

provision of services to people with aphasia. International

Journal of Stroke, 8(Suppl1), 44.

Code, C., & Herrmann, M. (2003). The relevance

of emotional and psychosocial factors in aphasia to

rehabilitation. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation: An

International Journal, 13(1–2), 109–132.

Gialanella, B., Bertolinelli, M., Lissi, M., & Prometti,

P. (2011). Predicting outcome after stroke: The role of

aphasia. Disability and Rehabilitation, 33, 122–129.

Gold, S. J. (1983). Getting well: impression management

as stroke rehabilitation. Qualitative Sociology, 6, 238–254.

Hersh, D. (1998). Beyond the “plateau”: Discharge

dilemmas in chronic aphasia. Aphasiology, 12(3), 207–218.

Hersh, D. (2003). Experiences of treatment termination

in chronic aphasia. (Unpublished PhD Thesis). Flinders

University, Adelaide.

Kayes, N., Mudge, S., Bright, F. A. S., & McPherson, K.

(2015). Whose behavior matters? Rethinking practitioner

behaviour and its influence on rehabilitation outcomes.

In K. McPherson, B. E. Gibson, & A. Leplège. Rethinking

rehabilitation: Theory and practice, (pp. 249–271). Boca

Raton, FL: CRC Press.

Lazar, R. M. & Antoniello, D. (2008). Variability in recovery

from aphasia. Current Neurology and Neuroscience

Reports, 8, 497–502.

and cultural diversity, and family supports. This list is of

interest considering the evidence cited above that only

stroke severity, lesion size, and location have been shown

in the research literature to be predictive for recovery. In

practice, however, the clinicians interviewed in both studies

felt that a much wider range of issues was important to

consider. Motivation was frequently mentioned as a critical

factor in decisions about potential to benefit from therapy,

how much therapy to offer, and in discharge decisions.

Motivation was very closely tied to improvement,

enthusiasm, and the ability to set goals and to do the

required work to support reaching those goals. Being

motivated and making change were viewed as cyclical, and

if people were judged as lacking motivation, they were not

retained for therapy. There was some suggestion in Study

1 that people judged to be motivated would be seen more

frequently in the acute setting, and suggestions from both

studies that client motivation impacted on the quality of

the interaction in that it was more satisfying to work with

someone who was stimulating, interested, and enthusiastic.

The degree to which families were viewed as motivated to

support their family member with aphasia was also reported

as important for a successful outcome. Interestingly, some

SLPs commented on potentially unreasonable pressures

on their clients to remain motivated at a stressful time

when other physical and emotional concerns might be

overwhelming.

Discussion

The data presented in this paper confirms that SLPs make

judgments about motivation in their patients with aphasia

(along with many other factors), that these judgments

influence what is done in therapy, and for how long it is

offered. SLPs do consider motivation in their predictions

about someone’s potential in rehabilitation. Having

motivation appears to be an expected behaviour in a

rehabilitation context that requires client participation and

enthusiasm for therapeutic success (Gold, 1983). Clearly,

this is within a context where people with aphasia may not

fully understand their own condition, the nature of their

treatment, or expectations for rehabilitation. They may be

facing considerable communicative, physical, cognitive,

psychological, perceptual, and emotional changes including

mood issues and fatigue, which together may impact on

their ability to demonstrate the energy and enthusiasm their

SLPs are looking for. Moreover, anxious family members

may not be aware of how crucial their contribution might be

to the overall impression of motivation for the therapist.

These are very subtle issues; clinical judgments which are

difficult to explain or make explicit. And yet, they contribute

to the decision-making within rehabilitation.

Of interest also are the comments in our results about

motivated patients leaving SLPs feeling energized. This

reciprocity of relationship has been raised by Kayes, Mudge,

Bright, and McPherson (2015) and Bright, Kayes, Worrall,

and McPherson (2015) in their discussion of engagement in

rehabilitation. These authors note that engagement, which

overlaps with the concept of motivation discussed here,

can be viewed both as a patient state but also as a

co-constructed process reflecting the quality of the

relationship between the clinician and patient. Effectively,

the work by Kayes et al. (2015) and Bright et al. (2015)

suggests that a therapist who fails to engage a patient well

(perhaps because of ineffective goal setting, poorly chosen

therapy tasks and materials, or insufficient rapport and

relationship building) will perhaps then judge that patient as