Previous Page  20 / 68 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 20 / 68 Next Page
Page Background

18

S

p eech

P

athology

A

ustralia

MULTICULTURALISM AND DYSPHAGIA

periods of reflection and integrating new information or

experiences as they came to hand. It involved explicitly

representing our existing but tacit beliefs about language,

which were based on our professional experience as much as

on research findings. Over a two-year period, we gradually

developed what we have called our Conceptual Model of

Language and Related Constructs (Walsh, 2007; Walsh &

Stassi, 2007) (Figure 5 on page 20). The model is necessarily

complex, reflecting the nature of language. The following

sections explore individual aspects of the model.

The internal language system

Within our model, the internal language system in the brain is

considered as a constantly self-organising, extremely complex

system of symbolic representations that carry all information

about an individual concept. Language development pro­

gresses over time from early single idiosyncratic symbols with

some lexical (word) features, to symbols grouped according

to certain lexical features, to conventional symbols within

complex (and overlapping) hierarchies, to multiple and

complex linking within hierarchies and other organisational

systems, as illustrated in figure 2.

disconnected from the real world. He also said that even

commonly used terms for which professionals think that they

share definitions are used with a range of meanings and

applications. Wilson went on to suggest that debates about

precision and accuracy in definitions are often dismissed as

“nit-picking”.

Stage one of our journey concluded with recognition that

language

was used variably within the profession, either because

it refers to a complex multifaceted phenomenon, or because it

refers to a group of different phenomena. A single sentence

definition may not be possible for something so complex. We

began to consider whether a conceptual model may be a

better tool to explore possible definitions of language.

Conceptual models of language

Conceptual models represent and delineate the facets of

complex constructs, and relationships between these facets. Con­

ceptual models provide a framework for reasoning. While they

are idealised and abstract, they are evaluated on the basis of

their coherence with experience and their usefulness in explain­

ing the world (Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, 2006).

Wilson (2005, p. 73) stated: “What we do is driven by how we

think, and how we think is largely determined by the con­

cepts we use.” Each individual’s conceptual model underpins

what they do and what they believe their role is (Wallach &

Ehren, 2004). Figure 1 represents the importance of conceptual

models for practice and the role of the professional.

Role

Practice

Conceptual

model

Concepts and beliefs

Behaviour and outcomes

Expectations and

satisfaction

Figure 1. Conceptual model and professional practice

The second stage of the journey, therefore, was to look for a

conceptual model which precisely articulated all the facets of

language. Searches through speech pathology and related

professional literature yielded a number of processing models

(which explained “how” language worked), but these models

did not explain “what” language actually “was”. Eventually we

foundDuchan’s (2006, p. 736) illuminating statement: “Conceptual

frameworks are easy to ignore; like the air we breathe, their

presence is everywhere, once they are looked for. Yet they are

often taken for granted, underestimated and under-examined.”

It seemed, as Duchan suggested, that the speech pathology

profession had perhaps taken for granted and not sufficiently

examined one of its central concepts. The only viable next

step on our journey was to articulate and examine our own

conceptual model of language.

Articulating our personal

conceptual model of language

Articulating our personal conceptual model was an extended

stop-start process involving long stimulating discussions,

Time and increasing complexity

Figure 2. The internal language system organisation over time

As a child develops, more “information” is built into the

symbolic representation for a concept (Shelton & Caramazza,

1999). Over time, visual, tactile, semantic, morphemic, likely

occurrence, situational, syntactic, pragmatic, spoken word

features (phonological), written word features (orthographic)

and other features are added. The addition of new information

allows the continual creation of new and/or more complex

organisational systems of symbols. In other words, a major

reorganisation of storage and links between symbols takes

place. Our conception of the internal language system is

therefore quite unlike a static filing system or a dictionary.

In figure 2, each shaded rectangle represents a concept. In

early life, each concept is stored as a single entity without

organisation, but as the number of concepts/words grows,

basic organisation takes place: grouping words into, for

example, semantic categories, i.e., “horse” is an animal, or

“horse” is linked with concepts of situational co-occurrence

(“horse” + “in the field” but not “in the lounge”); (Yeh &

Barsalou, 2006). Over time, hierarchical and/or parallel

organisation takes place, so that words are stored with

preferential links to other words (Shelton & Caramazza,

1999), e.g., “horse” may be linked to a farm animal category,

or links are developed between appropriate “noun + verb”

combinations (e.g., horse + neigh). Over time these conceptual

reorganisations and preferential links are influenced by newly

learned features of the word, such as information about how

a word/word class is pluralised. As the child grows, phono­

logical awareness influences organisation. For example, “horse”

might be preferentially linked or organised into a group of

words with the onset /h/ or those with the rime /

Os

/. As the

child develops and interacts with the world, lexical

organisation is based on environmental demands and priorities.

For example, learning to spell influences organisation; once a

child has learnt sufficient written words, the brain undergoes

a “lexical reorganisation” to favour retrieval of words accord