Previous Page  177 / 218 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 177 / 218 Next Page
Page Background

175

General discussion

ADHDwho take methylphenidate on a daily basis and who refrained from taking their normal

medication prior to the experimental session. When comparing this to drug administration

studies in non-medicated subjects, the long term effect of methylphenidate should be taken

into account. For example, it has been suggested that long-term use of methylphenidate may

– among other things – decrease excessive reward-related signalling in the striatum (Seeman

and Madras, 1998; Robbins, 2002). It remains unclear from

chapter 4

to what extent the

effects in the ADHD patients after medication withdrawal reflect deficits related to ADHD, or

to long term effect of methylphenidate use. Another limitation of the work in

chapter 4

is that

it failed to reveal clear behavioural effects, i.e. it remains unclear how the change in striatal

signalling would affect behaviour.

The results in

chapter 5

speak to opposite effects of dopamine on cognitive focusing (task

repetition) and cognitive flexibility (task switching). However, cognitive flexibility and stability

were not formally manipulated in this study. Moreover given the age-related degeneration of

dopamine neurons, and the opposite effects of reward on task switching and repeat trials, it

is reasonable to speculate that dopamine mediated these opposite reward-related effects on

cognitive control. However, future work should test this hypothesis (

future research

).

In

chapter 6

I present a novel rodent paradigm allowing the independent manipulation of

reward motivation and cognitive control, enabling the assessment of the interaction between

reward and task switching. Although this paradigm parallels the paradigm used in human

subjects (

box 2.3

), at least two differences should be considered when comparing the results

between species. First, whereas the size of the reward varied on a trial-by-trial basis in the

human version, the rodents performed the task-switching paradigm in separate high and

low reward blocks. However, similar block-designs have been used in humans to reveal

motivation–cognition interactions (Locke and Braver, 2008; Kouneiher et al., 2009), although

not with the rewarded task-switching paradigm. It is therefore unlikely that these differences in

trial-by-trial fluctuations in reward expectancy will have dissociable effects on task-switching

behaviour. Second, the trial duration in the rodent paradigm is much larger than that in the

human paradigm: The animals were presented with the task cue for one minute and were then

given another minute to press the levers during the presentation of the target, whereas these

events lasted merely a second in the human paradigm. As a consequence, the time between

the task cue and the target was much shorter in the human version of the paradigm than it

was in the rodent paradigm (i.e. 400ms instead of 1 minute). Longer cue-target intervals in

humans are known to decrease the switch cost (Meiran et al., 2000). Moreover, situations in

real-life often require instantaneous decisions and the need for lengthy preparation times

can be detrimental for survival. In the future it is thus advisable to reduce the the cue-target

interval. The large cue-target interval in chapter 6 may potentially account for a switch benefit

in the high reward condition, and a non-significant switch cost in the low reward condition:

If increasing the cue-target interval will increase the difficulty of switching, animals may well

show the same pattern of results as that observed in the 9R subjects in the healthy population.

One additional question that is raised when comparing the results in rodents to those in