Previous Page  174 / 218 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 174 / 218 Next Page
Page Background

172

Chapter 8

aspects of flexible control by analysis of the types of errors: Animals may fail to shift (i.e. they

persevere on the old rule) or they may exhibit deficits in rule maintenance (i.e. they initially

shift, but then fail to maintain this new strategy). One draw-back, when trying to compare the

results obtained with this paradigm to those observed with the human cued task-switching

paradigm is that switches are not signalled by a cue, but need to be learned. In addition, set-

shifting paradigms only allow one switch per test, rather than testing the fast trial-by-trial

adaptation generally measured in humans.

Extensive studies on strategy set-shifting have revealed a prominent role for the rodent medial

PFC, in particular the prelimbic cortex (Ragozzino et al., 1999; Floresco et al., 2006b; Floresco

et al., 2008a). In addition, dopamine signalling plays an important role in successful shifting

behaviour (Floresco et al., 2006b; Haluk and Floresco, 2009), as does the dorsal striatum

(Ragozzino et al., 2002). Human cued task switching work generally reports neural responses

in the dorsal striatum and dorsal prefrontal cortex (inferior frontal gyrus) and a role for

dopamine (Crone et al., 2006; Stelzel et al., 2010; Aarts et al., 2012; Aarts et al., 2014a), (see

Derrfuss et al., 2005) for a meta-analysis.

An interesting open question relates to if and how strategy set-shifting differs from cued task

switching. A particularly important distinction resides in the learning component. Whereas

the strategy set-shifting paradigm assesses how well animals

learn

to change their behaviour;

cued switching requires the ability to quickly change task-sets. Given the role for the nucleus

accumbens in learning (Schultz et al., 1997), it is perhaps not surprising that the rodent

nucleus accumbens is crucial for certain aspects of successful strategy shifting (Floresco et

al., 2006a). Interestingly, this nucleus is not involved in the initial shift, a role reserved for

the dorsal striatum, but it is crucial to maintain a recently introduced novel rule. The first

study that tested cued task switching in rodents (Baker and Ragozzino, 2014b, a) revealed

that flexible behaviour was disrupted by lesions of the medial PFC and the basal ganglia,

including the dorsal striatum. However, one major confound in this paradigm is caused

by the occurrence of ~4 times more repeat than switch trials. As a consequence, switching

between task-sets is no longer the only difference between switch and repeat trials. Instead,

potential confounds are introduced (e.g. increased novelty or saliency of switch trials, and the

overall expectation that a task will repeat). This confound is not present in the novel paradigm

presented in

chapter 6

where trials switch and repeat equally often.

Together, the work in human subjects and in rodents both show an important role for

dopamine, the dorsal striatum and corticostriatal circuit in task-switching, in particular

the inferior frontal gyrus in humans and the prelimbic cortex in rodents (Robbins, 2007;

Klanker et al., 2013), suggesting that these processes translate quite well across species. So

far it is unclear how the role of the nucleus accumbens in strategy set-shifting (Floresco et

al., 2006a) can be reconciled with its role in motivated cognitive control (

chapter 6

). One

possibility may be related to the role of this region in the approach of reward-related stimuli

and its role in facilitating appropriate actions. The nucleus accumbens may play a role in how

much updating takes place in prefrontal cortex, thereby mediating a balance between flexible