Previous Page  11 / 56 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 11 / 56 Next Page
Page Background www.speechpathologyaustralia.org.au

JCPSLP

Volume 15, Number 1 2013

9

start of the subsequent session, preschoolers engaged only

in

sentence build-up

(“Put it all together”), where the

subject+verb+object was combined to produce a

grammatically correct sentence (sentence point). Sentence-

breakdown was not required once the 80% criterion was

achieved. Random observations (20% of sessions) by

observers using a checklist of critical intervention elements

(e.g., the intervention procedure, identification of the

interventionist, the session number, group type, length of

the session, and the techniques used during the session)

revealed that intervention fidelity was maintained 100% of

the time. See Appendix for a sample intervention routine.

For computer-assisted intervention, expressive grammar

training was completed using

My Sentence Builder

. This

program is embedded within a syntactic slot-filler approach

with visual representations for semantic and grammatical

elements provided using picture support. Using a drill-play

approach with modelling and repetition, preschooler–SLP

dyads moved from screen-to-screen, selecting components

during sentence-breakdown. The SLP took preschoolers

to the sentence-creation screen first and told them

they would be “making up” things about boys and girls.

The dyad progressed through the subject, verb, object

selection screens to choose the subject, verb, object for

placement in the sentence-box at the bottom of the screen.

Preschoolers were then asked to “put it all together”.

Following a correct production, preschoolers were taken

to the sentence-selection and animation-production

screens. The slow, deliberate, and sequential selection

used with computer-based visual representations was the

key intervention difference between computer-assisted and

table-top intervention.

For table-top intervention, preschoolers engaged in

clinician–client dyads for the same multi-step intervention

procedures, this time using typical table-top materials (e.g.,

books, felt, or paper dollhouse objects) to demonstrate

the semantic elements within the same drill-play activities.

Emphatic stress was included to increase the salience

of sentence components (grammatical and semantic)

in contrast to the computer-based syntactic slot-filler

approach. This technique involved the SLP verbally

stressing sentence components during the multi-

step procedure. In comparison to computer-assisted

intervention, consistent visual support demonstrating

grammatical elements was not provided. Instead, visual

support was provided using table-top materials for

semantic elements.

Preschoolers in the waitlist control group did not receive

expressive grammar intervention from the SLP during the

study. At the end of the study, intervention was offered.

Design and analysis

A pre-post-follow-up design was employed. The secondary

analysis of the Washington et al. (2011) data was

completed using two mixed model multivariate analyses of

variance (MANOVAs) with pre-set alphas (

p

< .05). Effect

sizes “an estimate of the effect of intervention” (Portney &

Watkins, 2009, p. 373), represented by eta squared (

N

2

)

and partial eta squared (

N

p

2

), were also reported. The first

MANOVA compared the three groups (computer-assisted,

table-top, no intervention: between-subjects factor) for DSS

and MLU change scores (dependent variables) for the

3-month gain1, 3-month gain2 and 6-month gain (i.e., gain

period, within-subjects factor). The second MANOVA

compared the three groups for DSS per cent error rates

(number of incorrect attempts for personal pronoun, main

verb and number of utterances not awarded a sentence

grammatical categories established by Lee (1974) that

indicate grammar development and complexity in young

children (i.e., indefinite pronouns/noun modifier, personal

pronouns, main verbs, secondary verbs, negatives,

conjunctions, interrogative reversals, wh-questions).

Growth beyond the 10th percentile (pre-test

performance) to “within normal limits” represented clinically

meaningful growth (i.e., acceleration) in spontaneous

grammar skills. Since participants were 3- to 4-years of

age (

M

= 4;4), grammatical performance was compared

to the DSS point growth expected over a 6-month

period for typical 4-year-olds (Lee & Canter, 1971). The

50th percentile was used as the expectation for normal

developmental change, representing a 0.76 point-gain for

this age group (Lee & Canter, 1971). Clinically meaningful

DSS growth was established at or greater than the 0.76

DSS point-gain for change between pre-intervention to

post-intervention (3-month gain1), post-intervention to

3 months post-intervention (3-month gain2), and pre-

intervention to 3 months post-intervention (6-month gain).

To establish DSS scoring reliability, 10% of language

samples were randomly chosen for analysis by graduate

SLP students. Inter-rater reliability for DSS, including point-

to-point comparisons for word transcription, appropriate

DSS sentences, category, and scoring was 91.3%, 97.2%,

90.8%, and 90.8%, respectively.

Preschoolers’ mean length of utterance (MLU; Brown,

1973; Miller, 1981) was also calculated for the same language

samples. MLU is another useful measure of grammatical

morphology that offers information about use of morphemes

and developmental change, but is limited in capturing

changes in grammatical complexity (Goffman & Leonard,

2000). Due to this limitation, (a) MLU change scores were

calculated to determine if gains in use of morphemes

co-occurred with gains in grammatical complexity as

measured by the DSS and (b) the 0.76 criterion applied to

the DSS change scores was not applied to the MLU change

scores. At pre-test, a univariate ANOVA revealed non

significant between-group differences in MLU (

p

= .140).

An analysis of DSS per cent error rates for number of

incorrect attempts for the

personal pronoun

,

main verb

,

and number of utterances not awarded a

sentence point

,

representing targeted grammatical categories, was also

completed. There were no significant differences on

per cent error rates between the three groups at pre-

intervention for

personal pronoun

(

p

= .501),

main verb

(

p

= .072), and

sentence point

(

p

= .081). Errors in these

categories decrease with time for typically developing

preschoolers (Lee, 1974) and show improvement with

intervention for children with language impairment (Fey,

Cleave, Long, & Hughes, 1993; Lee, Koenigsknecht, &

Mulgern, 1975).

Intervention

Preschoolers receiving intervention in the Washington et al.

(2011) study participated in 20-minute sessions once

weekly for 10 weeks with an SLP, typical of the intensity

and frequency of intervention services under the

government-funded initiative. The following procedure was

used: (a) a 2-to-7-minute practice block introduced the

routine; (b)

sentence-breakdown

was used to individually

elicit sentence components (subject-noun phrase, verb,

object-noun phrase) utilising the following questions, “Who

do you want to play with?” (subject), “What is s/he doing?”

(verb), and “What does s/he want to play with?” (object); (c)

sessions followed the same procedure until 80% accuracy

over two consecutive sessions was achieved; and (d) at the