Previous Page  13 / 56 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 13 / 56 Next Page
Page Background www.speechpathologyaustralia.org.au

JCPSLP

Volume 15, Number 1 2013

11

The negative changes observed in DSS change scores

for preschoolers in the waitlist control group on average

were indicative of unsuccessful attempts at creating

grammatically complex and correct productions and not

a regression in these skills. An examination of the per

cent error rates revealed that these children attempted

to produce accurate and complex personal pronouns,

main verbs and achieve the sentence points, but were

unsuccessful in these attempts, resulting in the higher

per cent error rates compared to cohorts in intervention.

Lee (1974) and Lee and Canter (1971) suggested that as

children‘s language skills develop, unsuccessful attempts

at more complex productions in spontaneous language

are expected. Thus, errors in syntactic productions are

a normal part of grammatical development for young

children with typically developing language skills. In fact for

5-year-olds, a 0.12-point decline in DSS scores at the 50th

percentile is expected in 6 months. Since these errors are

anticipated for typical preschoolers, errors for preschoolers

who do not have typical language skills and are not

receiving intervention would not be unexpected.

Clinical implications

The current findings expand on those reported in the

Washington et al. (2011) study by providing evidence that

grammatical language interventions were associated with

accelerated growth to “within normal limits” for grammar

development. The inclusion of computer-assisted and

table-top intervention techniques, including specific

step-by-step procedures, may be important in achieving

this growth during SLP-led interventions for this population.

Alternatively, preschoolers with SLI who do not receive

intervention are at a significantly greater risk for not

achieving good outcomes.

Limitations and future directions

Participants included in this study demonstrated specific

expressive grammar deficits, thus application of these

findings to other preschoolers with SLI with receptive

difficulties is limited. The mixed evidence regarding the

effectiveness of expressive grammar intervention for

children with concomitant receptive and expressive

grammatical deficits (cf. Law et al., 2012) suggests

additional research is needed.

Another limitation is that DSS techniques are time

consuming and scores are narrower in their representation

of language skills (Lee & Canter, 1971). The inclusion of

MLU in this study offered another measure of linguistic

gains that might not have been captured in DSS scores.

Future research with other children with SLI with receptive

language deficits should expand the measures used to

establish acceleration in expressive grammar and perhaps

also consider sampling beyond a clinical conversation with

an adult.

Conclusion

Expressive grammar intervention offers a therapeutic

advantage over no intervention for the enhanced

development of spontaneous language skills in

preschoolers with expressive language impairment.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the families, children,

speech-language pathologists, graduate SLP students and

research assistants who made this research possible. This

study was funded by the Canadian Language and Literacy

Research Network, the Ontario Graduate Scholarship, the

at the pre-set adjusted alpha level (

p

< .017) were

conducted. There was a significant interaction effect for

group X time for DSS per cent error rates for,

personal

pronoun

,

F

(4,62) = 7.05,

p

< .001,

N

p

2

= .31,

main verb

,

F

(4,62) = 7.44,

p

< .001,

N

p

2

= .32 and

sentence point

,

F

(4,62) = 8.08,

p

< .001,

N

p

2

= .34.

Simple main effect tests were completed as a follow-up

to the significant interaction for each ANOVA. Findings for

DSS per cent error rates in each grammatical category met

the set significance level (

p

< .006) for follow-up tests at

post-intervention and 3 months post-intervention. Pairwise

comparisons of means revealed that computer-assisted

and table-top intervention facilitated significantly lower

DSS per cent error rates in each category compared to

no-intervention. The two intervention groups did not differ

statistically (

p

> .05). See Figure 3.

Discussion

Preschoolers with SLI who received expressive grammar

intervention experienced significantly greater improvement

in their grammar skills that were maintained at post-

intervention and at 3 months post-intervention compared to

waitlist controls. Maintenance of gains beyond the

intervention period is considered an important intervention

outcome indicative of development (Yoder et al., 2011). The

magnitude of gain in 6 months as a result of expressive

grammar intervention was above that expected for typically

developing 4-year-olds at the 50th percentile,

demonstrating significant accelerated growth beyond the

starting point in intervention. Thus, intervention offered a

therapeutic advantage over no intervention for facilitating

the outcomes observed for grammatical complexity, use of

morphemes, and accuracy in targeted grammatical

categories. However, computer-assisted and table-top

intervention resulted in similar effects on the spontaneous

use of expressive grammar skills. Consequently, it is

important to consider the role of expressive grammar

intervention, regardless of type, for facilitating growth in

these skills.

The goals of language intervention for grammatical

deficits are to improve children’s production and

comprehension of targeted language forms (Leonard et

al., 2006). By directly targeting children’s grammatical

use of language forms, they become more aware of how

to accurately sequence morphemes and phonemes

into meaningful units. The explicit support and attention

to grammatical features included in intervention may

provide more time to process information and increase

preschoolers’ awareness and decrease language-learning

efforts during expressive grammar intervention.

During computer-assisted and table-top intervention,

the SLP made deliberate attempts to highlight sentence

components needed to produce grammatically correct

sentences. The syntactic slot-filler approach and

emphatic stress were equally effective in highlighting these

components for the sample of preschoolers. Implementing

these techniques in a drill-play format with modelling and

repetition resulted in multiple opportunities for focused

practice (3000 production opportunities for the two

interventions). Previous researchers highlight the necessity

of explicit, repeated exposures to target forms that address

grammatical productions for this population (Cleave &

Rice, 1997). Computer-assisted intervention may provide

motivation and increased tolerance for repetition in some

preschoolers with SLI and should be considered a viable

alternative to table-top intervention, where appropriate (cf.

Washington et al., 2011).