130
ACQ
Volume 13, Number 3 2011
ACQ
uiring Knowledge in Speech, Language and Hearing
References
Berry, R., & Hudson, J. (1997).
Making the jump: A
resource book for teachers of Aboriginal students
. Broome,
WA: Catholic Education Office.
Butcher, A. (2008). Linguistic aspects of Australian
Aboriginal English.
Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics
,
22
(8),
625–642. doi: 10.1080/02699200802223535
Centre for Community Child Health and Telethon Institute
for Child Health Research. (2009).
A snapshot of early
childhood development in Australia – AEDI national report
2009
. Canberra, ACT: Australian Government.
de Plevitz, L. (2006). Special schooling for Indigenous
students: A new form of racial discrimination.
Australian
Journal of Indigenous Education
,
35
(1), 44–53.
Epstein, S., & Phillips, J. (2009). Storytelling skills
of children with specific language impairment.
Child
Language Teaching and Therapy
,
25
(3), 285–300.
doi:10.1177/0265659009339819
Fey, M.E., Catts, H.W., Proctor-Williams, K., Tomblin,
J.B., & Zhang, X. (2004). Oral and written story composition
skills of children with language impairment.
Journal of
Speech Language, and Hearing Research
,
47
(6), 1301–
1318. doi: 1092-4388/04/4706-1301
Freelon, D. (2011). ReCal:
Reliability calculation for the
masses
. Retrieved from
http://dfreelon.org/.Gould, J. (2001). Joint assessments for Koori children:
Working with Aboriginal people to improve outcomes for
Aboriginal English (Lingo) speaking Aboriginal children in
the speech pathology assessment situation.
ACQuiring
Knowledge in Speech, Language and Hearing
,
3
(3),
136–138.
Heilmann, J., Miller, J.F., Nockerts, A., & Dunaway, C.
(2010). Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme: Using
narrative retells in young school-age children.
American
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology
,
19
(2), 154–166.
doi:10.1044/1058-0360(2009/08-0024)
Hoff, E., & Tian, C. (2005). Socioeconomic status and
cultural influences on language.
Journal of Communication
Disorders
,
38
, 271–278.
Malcolm, I., Haig, Y., Konigsberg, P., Rochecouste, J.,
Collard, G., Hill, A., & Cahill, R. (1999).
Two-way English:
Towards more user-friendly education for speakers of
Aboriginal English
. East Perth, WA: Education Department
of Western Australia.
Marinis, T., & Chondrogianni, V. (2010). Production of
tense marking in successive bilingual children: When do
they converge with their monolingual peers?
International
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology
,
12
(1), 19–28. doi:
10.3109/17549500903434125
Mayer, M. (1969).
Frog, where are you?
New York: Puffin
Pied Piper.
Miller, J. F., & Iglesias, A. (2008).
Systematic Analysis
of Language Transcripts (SALT)
, Research Version 2008
[Computer software]. Madison, WI: Language Analysis
Laboratory.
Muñoz, M.L., Gillam, R.B., Peña, E.D., & Gulley-Faehnle,
A. (2003). Measures of language development in fictional
narratives of Latino children.
Language, Speech, and
Hearing Services in Schools
,
34
(4), 332–342. doi: 0161-
1461/03/3404-0332
Moses, K., & Wigglesworth, G. (2008). The silence of
the frogs: Dysfunctional discourse in the “English-only”
Aboriginal classroom. In J. Simpson & G. Wigglesworth
(Eds.),
Children’s language and multilingualism: Indigenous
language use at home and school
(pp. 129–153). New
York: Continuum International Publishing Group.
at the beginning of every utterance. Possible reasons why
this participant may have underperformed include tiredness,
unfamiliarity with the task or shyness towards the examiner.
The “shame” response that may arise in testing situations
with Aboriginal children has been well reported and may
also have been a factor here (Gould, 2001; Malcolm et al.,
1999). Feelings of “shame” may lead to a reluctance to
speak or desire to leave a situation. Shame may arise from
situations where a person does not know what is expected
of them or feels singled out from the group.
Interesting trends are apparent when the results from
P#3 are excluded. Most other participants did well with
the Introduction, Character development and Conclusion
components. This is consistent with the observation that
people and place are of key importance in Aboriginal
storytelling (Malcolm et al., 1999). It is unclear why most
participants had difficulty describing the Mental states of
the story characters (feelings and thoughts). Referencing,
Cohesion, and Conflict resolution were challenging
components for the younger children but older children had
good mastery of these. This suggests that experience with
narratives at school contributed to the ability of Aboriginal
children to produce more mature oral narratives as
measured by the NSS.
Conclusion
This study was a preliminary exploration of oral narrative in
Aboriginal children. As such, the study is limited in several
ways including its small sample size and scope, and limited
information about the home languages and hearing status
of the children. However, the findings lead to some
important clinical implications. Speech pathologists need to
consider the language background of the Aboriginal
children they assess (including dialects, creoles, and
traditional languages) as well as information about the
child’s level of exposure to SAE. Until reliable normative
data are available, our ability to use grammatical or oral
narrative microstructure measures to define LI in Aboriginal
children is limited. There may also be greater benefit from
using measures that are more independent of grammar and
linguistic variation such as oral narrative structure.
The development of Australian English normative data
is needed as a focus of ongoing studies. Information is
needed on the language development of Aboriginal, Torres
Strait Islander, and non-Indigenous children, across the
range of remote, rural and urban contexts, language groups
and socioeconomic backgrounds. In addition, exploration
of appropriate assessment and language sampling
practices for Indigenous Australian children is required to
ensure optimum accommodation of cultural differences in
communication style (Gould, 2008). These differences may
include the need for relationship and purpose within the
interaction, avoidance of display questions, acceptance
of silence or a non-response, and strategies to minimise
“shame”. Until we have better normative data, clinicians are
advised to collect and analyse language samples over time
in order to monitor progress in response to intervention.
Acknowledgments
This paper is based on the honours project work of the
second author, supervised by the first author. Appreciation
is extended to the children who participated in this study
and to the school staff members, parents and caregivers
who supported them.