Previous Page  28 / 60 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 28 / 60 Next Page
Page Background

130

ACQ

Volume 13, Number 3 2011

ACQ

uiring Knowledge in Speech, Language and Hearing

References

Berry, R., & Hudson, J. (1997).

Making the jump: A

resource book for teachers of Aboriginal students

. Broome,

WA: Catholic Education Office.

Butcher, A. (2008). Linguistic aspects of Australian

Aboriginal English.

Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics

,

22

(8),

625–642. doi: 10.1080/02699200802223535

Centre for Community Child Health and Telethon Institute

for Child Health Research. (2009).

A snapshot of early

childhood development in Australia – AEDI national report

2009

. Canberra, ACT: Australian Government.

de Plevitz, L. (2006). Special schooling for Indigenous

students: A new form of racial discrimination.

Australian

Journal of Indigenous Education

,

35

(1), 44–53.

Epstein, S., & Phillips, J. (2009). Storytelling skills

of children with specific language impairment.

Child

Language Teaching and Therapy

,

25

(3), 285–300.

doi:10.1177/0265659009339819

Fey, M.E., Catts, H.W., Proctor-Williams, K., Tomblin,

J.B., & Zhang, X. (2004). Oral and written story composition

skills of children with language impairment.

Journal of

Speech Language, and Hearing Research

,

47

(6), 1301–

1318. doi: 1092-4388/04/4706-1301

Freelon, D. (2011). ReCal:

Reliability calculation for the

masses

. Retrieved from

http://dfreelon.org/.

Gould, J. (2001). Joint assessments for Koori children:

Working with Aboriginal people to improve outcomes for

Aboriginal English (Lingo) speaking Aboriginal children in

the speech pathology assessment situation.

ACQuiring

Knowledge in Speech, Language and Hearing

,

3

(3),

136–138.

Heilmann, J., Miller, J.F., Nockerts, A., & Dunaway, C.

(2010). Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme: Using

narrative retells in young school-age children.

American

Journal of Speech-Language Pathology

,

19

(2), 154–166.

doi:10.1044/1058-0360(2009/08-0024)

Hoff, E., & Tian, C. (2005). Socioeconomic status and

cultural influences on language.

Journal of Communication

Disorders

,

38

, 271–278.

Malcolm, I., Haig, Y., Konigsberg, P., Rochecouste, J.,

Collard, G., Hill, A., & Cahill, R. (1999).

Two-way English:

Towards more user-friendly education for speakers of

Aboriginal English

. East Perth, WA: Education Department

of Western Australia.

Marinis, T., & Chondrogianni, V. (2010). Production of

tense marking in successive bilingual children: When do

they converge with their monolingual peers?

International

Journal of Speech-Language Pathology

,

12

(1), 19–28. doi:

10.3109/17549500903434125

Mayer, M. (1969).

Frog, where are you?

New York: Puffin

Pied Piper.

Miller, J. F., & Iglesias, A. (2008).

Systematic Analysis

of Language Transcripts (SALT)

, Research Version 2008

[Computer software]. Madison, WI: Language Analysis

Laboratory.

Muñoz, M.L., Gillam, R.B., Peña, E.D., & Gulley-Faehnle,

A. (2003). Measures of language development in fictional

narratives of Latino children.

Language, Speech, and

Hearing Services in Schools

,

34

(4), 332–342. doi: 0161-

1461/03/3404-0332

Moses, K., & Wigglesworth, G. (2008). The silence of

the frogs: Dysfunctional discourse in the “English-only”

Aboriginal classroom. In J. Simpson & G. Wigglesworth

(Eds.),

Children’s language and multilingualism: Indigenous

language use at home and school

(pp. 129–153). New

York: Continuum International Publishing Group.

at the beginning of every utterance. Possible reasons why

this participant may have underperformed include tiredness,

unfamiliarity with the task or shyness towards the examiner.

The “shame” response that may arise in testing situations

with Aboriginal children has been well reported and may

also have been a factor here (Gould, 2001; Malcolm et al.,

1999). Feelings of “shame” may lead to a reluctance to

speak or desire to leave a situation. Shame may arise from

situations where a person does not know what is expected

of them or feels singled out from the group.

Interesting trends are apparent when the results from

P#3 are excluded. Most other participants did well with

the Introduction, Character development and Conclusion

components. This is consistent with the observation that

people and place are of key importance in Aboriginal

storytelling (Malcolm et al., 1999). It is unclear why most

participants had difficulty describing the Mental states of

the story characters (feelings and thoughts). Referencing,

Cohesion, and Conflict resolution were challenging

components for the younger children but older children had

good mastery of these. This suggests that experience with

narratives at school contributed to the ability of Aboriginal

children to produce more mature oral narratives as

measured by the NSS.

Conclusion

This study was a preliminary exploration of oral narrative in

Aboriginal children. As such, the study is limited in several

ways including its small sample size and scope, and limited

information about the home languages and hearing status

of the children. However, the findings lead to some

important clinical implications. Speech pathologists need to

consider the language background of the Aboriginal

children they assess (including dialects, creoles, and

traditional languages) as well as information about the

child’s level of exposure to SAE. Until reliable normative

data are available, our ability to use grammatical or oral

narrative microstructure measures to define LI in Aboriginal

children is limited. There may also be greater benefit from

using measures that are more independent of grammar and

linguistic variation such as oral narrative structure.

The development of Australian English normative data

is needed as a focus of ongoing studies. Information is

needed on the language development of Aboriginal, Torres

Strait Islander, and non-Indigenous children, across the

range of remote, rural and urban contexts, language groups

and socioeconomic backgrounds. In addition, exploration

of appropriate assessment and language sampling

practices for Indigenous Australian children is required to

ensure optimum accommodation of cultural differences in

communication style (Gould, 2008). These differences may

include the need for relationship and purpose within the

interaction, avoidance of display questions, acceptance

of silence or a non-response, and strategies to minimise

“shame”. Until we have better normative data, clinicians are

advised to collect and analyse language samples over time

in order to monitor progress in response to intervention.

Acknowledgments

This paper is based on the honours project work of the

second author, supervised by the first author. Appreciation

is extended to the children who participated in this study

and to the school staff members, parents and caregivers

who supported them.