Previous Page  48 / 60 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 48 / 60 Next Page
Page Background

150

ACQ

Volume 13, Number 3 2011

ACQ

uiring Knowledge in Speech, Language and Hearing

problems across a range of conditions. For example,

Paradis et al. (2003) concluded that bilingual children with

specific language impairment were not “more impaired”

than their participants that were monolingual (p. 123). In

fact, Pikho et al. (2007) and Tsybina and Eriks-Brophy

(2010) both cite evidence that early acquisition of a second

language and regular use of two languages may have

beneficial effects on various cognitive abilities in young

children.

Drawing conclusions from the

available research

Although there is only a small body of evidence available to

answer the questions under review, the following

conclusions can be drawn: 1) results were positive,

end of the levels of evidence (Goldstein, 2006; Kohnert,

2010; Paradis, 2010; Thordardottir, 2010). Despite such

critique, however, the conclusions drawn were similar and

positive across the review papers. For example,

Thordardottir concluded “Although the current research

base is limited, the few studies available to date uniformly

suggest that interventions that include a focus on both

languages are superior to those that focus on only one

language” (Thordardottir, 2010, p. 523). Further, “no study

was found which showed a monolingual focus to be

superior to a bilingual one” (Thordardottir, 2010, p.524).

Similarly, Goldstein (2006) “did not find evidence suggesting

bilingual intervention was counter-indicated” (p. 318). More

specifically, these authors found no evidence that

bilingualism exposes children to greater risk of language

Table 2. Research articles identified

Articles identified

Purpose

Type of study. Credibility

of level

Tsybina & Eriks-Brophy (2010). Bilingual

Spanish-English bilingual children.

See detailed analysis in

dialogic book-reading intervention for

table 4

preschoolers with slow expressive

vocabulary development.

Seung, Siddiqi, & Elder (2006). Intervention Case study. 3-year-old Korean-English, diagnosed Autism Spectrum NHMRC – no level

outcomes of a bilingual child with autism.

Disorder. 12 months intervention in Korean, then 6 months English

introduced, then 6 months English-only . Made substantial gains in

Justice & Fey, 2004 –

both languages with “transition” between dominant languages.

Level III

Thordardottir, Elin, Ellis Weismer, & Smith.

Single-case experimental design. 4;6 year old Icelandic-English

NHMRC – no level

(1997). Vocabulary learning in bilingual and bilingual child, alternating treatments for vocabulary, monolingual

monolingual clinical intervention.

English and bilingual. Significant improvement in both conditions,

Justice & Fey, 2004 –

but better results in bilingual for home, but not school vocabulary.

Level II-3

Waltzman, Robbins, et al. (2003). Second

Eighteen profoundly hearing-impaired children who were reported to NHMRC –level III-3

oral language capabilities in children with

be bilingual received cochlear implantation at age 5 or younger.

cochlear implants.

On standard speech perception and receptive and expressive

Justice & Fey, 2004

language measures, some pediatric cochlear implant recipients

– Level II-3

showed competency in a second spoken language in addition to

their primary language. Majority showed age appropriate receptive

and/or expressive language in their primary language commensurate

with normal-hearing children.

Wauters, Knoors, Vervloed, & Aarnoutse.

16 children, 6–10 years old, at school for deaf, learning Dutch and speechBite

TM

classified

(2001). Sign facilitation in word recognition. sign language of the Netherlands. Taught written forms with only

this study as a Randomised

oral accompaniment, or oral and sign. Randomly assigned to

Control Trial, but scored it

conditions. Higher gains came from the speech + sign condition,

only 3/10

although not for all individuals.

Kay-Raining Bird, Cleave, Trudeau,

Compared the language abilities of 8 children with Down syndrome NHMRC –level III-1

Thordardottir, Sutton, & Thorpe. (2005).

being raised bilingually with those of 3 control groups matched on

The language abilities of bilingual children

developmental level: monolingual children with DS (n = 14),

Justice & Fey, 2004 –

with Down syndrome. (Not strictly an

monolingual typically developing (TD) children (n = 18), and bilingual

Level II-1

intervention study; included because of

TD children (n = 11). All children had at least 100 words in their

the population involved)

productive vocabularies but a mean length of utterance of less than

3.5. Found similar profile of language abilities in bilingual children

as for their monolingual peers. There was no evidence of a

detrimental effect of bilingualism, but considerable diversity in the

second-language abilities.

Table 3. Sample of speechBite

TM

search results

Authors

Title

Year

Method

Rating

Wauters et al.

Sign facilitation in word recognition

2001 Randomised controlled trial

03/10

Pihko et al.

Group intervention changes brain activity in bilingual

2007 Non-randomised controlled 03/10

language-impaired children

trial

Hammer et al.

Bilingual children’s language abilities and early reading

2007 Case series

N/A

outcomes in Head Start and kindergarten

Seung H, Siddiqi S, Elder JH Intervention outcomes of a bilingual child with autism

2006 Single-subject design

N/A